Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 May 28:7:69.
doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00069. eCollection 2013.

Consolidated learning can be susceptible to gradually-developing interference in prolonged motor learning

Affiliations

Consolidated learning can be susceptible to gradually-developing interference in prolonged motor learning

Yuko Yotsumoto et al. Front Comput Neurosci. .

Abstract

When multiple items are learned in sequential order, learning for one item tends to be disrupted by subsequently learned items. Such retrograde interference has been studied with paradigms conducted over a relatively short term. Resistance to interference is generally believed to be a measure of learning or consolidation. Here, we used a finger-tapping motor sequence paradigm to examine interference in prolonged motor learning. Three groups of nine subjects participated in training sessions for 16 days, and practiced three different sequences in different orders and combinations. We found that a well-trained motor sequence was subject to a gradual interference when the subsequent learning was paired in a particular order. The results suggest that a well-learned motor memory is still susceptible to interference, and that resistance to interference in one condition does not necessarily imply full, permanent consolidation.

Keywords: consolidation; finger tapping; interference; motor learning; retrograde interference.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Schematic representation of the experimental procedure.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Performance during a finger-tapping motor sequence task for three training sequences (three groups). The mean number of correctly typed responses as a function of the training day. In the first part of the training session (Days 1–8), a single sequence was trained; two sequences (sequences B and A) were practiced in all groups in the second training period (Days 9–16). (A) Group ABA; (B) Group CBA; (C) Group BBA.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Relative performance improvement compared to the initial day performance by group.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Averaged number of correctly tapped sequences (±s.e.m.) plotted for each block and for each session. For each group, the mean numbers of correctly tapped sequences are plotted for each learned sequence.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Within-session learning as indicated by an increase in the number of correctly tapped sequences within a session (±s.e.m.). Asterisks indicate sessions with significant deviation from zero (p < 0.05, Bonferroni correction).
Figure 6
Figure 6
(A) Correlations between the initial performance on Day 1 and the extent of interference. (B) Correlations between the performance improvement during the first part of the training, and the extent of interference.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alvarez P., Squire L. R. (1994). Memory consolidation and the medial temporal lobe: a simple network model. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 7041–7045 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Brashers-Krug T., Shadmehr R., Bizzi E. (1996). Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature 382, 252–255 10.1038/382252a0 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Censor N., Sagi D., Cohen L. G. (2012). Common mechanisms of human perceptual and motor learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 658–664 10.1038/nrn3315 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cohen D. A., Robertson E. M. (2011). Preventing interference between different memory tasks. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 953–955 10.1038/nn.2840 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Dayan E., Cohen L. G. (2011). Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron 72, 443–454 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.008 - DOI - PMC - PubMed