Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 May;87(5):052717.
doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.87.052717. Epub 2013 May 29.

How noise contributes to time-scale invariance of interval timing

Affiliations

How noise contributes to time-scale invariance of interval timing

Sorinel A Oprisan et al. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 2013 May.

Abstract

Time perception in the suprasecond range is crucial for fundamental cognitive processes such as decision making, rate calculation, and planning. In the vast majority of species, behavioral manipulations, and neurophysiological manipulations, interval timing is scale invariant: the time-estimation errors are proportional to the estimated duration. The origin and mechanisms of this fundamental property are unknown. We discuss the computational properties of a circuit consisting of a large number of (input) neural oscillators projecting on a small number of (output) coincidence detector neurons, which allows time to be coded by the pattern of coincidental activation of its inputs. We show that time-scale invariance emerges from the neural noise, such as small fluctuations in the firing patterns of its input neurons and in the errors with which information is encoded and retrieved by its output neurons. In this architecture, time-scale invariance is resistant to manipulations as it depends neither on the details of the input population nor on the distribution probability of noise.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIG. 1.
FIG. 1.. (Color online) Accurate and time-scale invariant interval timing.
(A) The response rate of rats timing a 30s interval (left) or 90s interval (right) overlap (center) when normalized by the maximum response rate (vertical axis), respectively, the criterion time (horizontal axis); redrawn from [8]. (B) Time-scale invariance in human subjects for 8s and 21s criteria; redrawn from [10]. (C) Systemic cocaine (COC) administration speeds-up timing proportional (scalar) to the original criteria 30s and 90s; redrawn from [8]. (D) The hemodynamic response associated with a subject’s active time reproduction scales with the timed criterion, 11s v. 17s; redrawn from [14].
FIG. 2.
FIG. 2.. (Color online) The neurobiological structures involved in interval timing and the corresponding simplified SBF architecture.
(A) Schematic representation of some neurobiological structures involved in interval timing. The color-coded connectivities among different areas emphasize appropriate neuromodulatory pathways. The two main areas involved in interval timing are frontal cortex (FC) and basal ganglia (BG). (B) In our implementation of the SBF model, the states of the N cortical oscillators (input neurons) at reinforcement time T are stored in the reference memory as a set of weights wi(T). During test trials, the working memory stores the current weights wi(t) and, together with the reference memory, projects its content onto spiny (output) neurons of the BG. FC: frontal cortex; MC: motor cortex; BG: basal ganglia; TH: thalamus. GPE: globus pallidus external; GPI: globus pallidus internal; STn: subthalamic nucleus; SNc/r: substantia nigra pars compacta/reticulata; VTA: ventral tegmental area; Glu: glutamate; DA: dopamine; GABA: gamma-aminobutyric acid; ACh: acetylcholine.
FIG. 3.
FIG. 3.. (Color online) A noise-free SBF model does not produce time-scale invariance.
(A) Analytically predicted (inset) and numerically generated output of a noise-free SBF-sin model with N = 100 for T = 5s and T = 15s. (B) As predicted analytically (theoretical), the width the output function of a noise free SBF-sin model is independent of the criterion time. (C) A noise-free SBF-ML model does not produce time-scale invariance either. The width of the Gaussian envelopes (dashed line for T = 5s and dashed-dotted line for t = 15s) remains constant.
FIG. 4.
FIG. 4.. (Color online) Time-scale invariance emerges from criterion time noise in the SBF model.
(A) Time-scale invariance emerges spontaneously in a nosy SBF-sin model; here the two criteria are T = 30s and T = 90s. The output functions (continuous lines) were fitted with Gaussian curves (dashed lines) in order to estimate the position of the peak and the width of the output function. (B) In an SBF-sin model, the standard deviation increases linearly with the criterion time in all four trials shown with different symbols. (C) In an SBF-ML implementation, low levels of Gaussian noise (solid rectangles represent σT = .1% and circles σT = 1%) produce an almost constant standard deviation of the output function similar to noise free case. At high enough levels of noise (solid triangles represent σT = 10%), the SBF-ML model with criterion time variance produces a standard deviation σo that increases linearly with the criterion time T, which is the hallmark of time-scale invariance.
FIG. 5.
FIG. 5.. (Color online) Time-scale invariance is robust to noise manipulation in the SBF model.
(A) In the presence of both criterion time and frequency variance, the SBF-ML produces accurate and time-scale invariant output. At the same time, multiple sources of noise determine a long tail in the output function, which is similar with experimental findings. (B) The scalar property is also preserved regardless the type of criterion time variability (uniform noise - solid triangles, Poisson noise - solid squares).

References

    1. Gallistel C, The organization of behavior (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1990).
    1. Catania AC, “Reinforcement schedules and psychophysical judgments: A study of some temporal properties of behavior,” in The theory of reinforcement schedules, edited by Schoenfeld W (Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1970) pp. 1–42.
    1. Roberts S, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 7, 242 (1981). - PubMed
    1. Buhusi C and Meck W, Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6, 755 (2005). - PubMed
    1. Mauk M and Buonomano D, Annu Rev Neurosci 27, 307 (2004). - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources