Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Jun;38(2):171-83.
doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2013.03.001. Epub 2013 Mar 16.

Behavioral inhibition and childhood stuttering

Affiliations

Behavioral inhibition and childhood stuttering

Dahye Choi et al. J Fluency Disord. 2013 Jun.

Abstract

Purpose: The purpose of this study was to assess the relation of behavioral inhibition to stuttering and speech/language output in preschool-age children who do (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS).

Method: Participants were preschool-age (ages 36-68 months), including 26 CWS (22 males) and 28 CWNS (13 males). Participants' behavioral inhibition (BI) was assessed by measuring the latency to their sixth spontaneous comment during conversation with an unfamiliar experimenter, using methodology developed by Kagan, Reznick, and Gibbons (1989). In addition to these measures of BI, each participant's stuttered and non-stuttered disfluencies and mean length of utterance (in morphemes) were assessed.

Results: Among the more salient findings, it was found that (1) there was no significant difference in BI between preschool-age CWS and CWNS as a group, (2) when extremely high versus low inhibited children were selected, there were more CWS with higher BI and fewer CWS with lower BI when compared to their CWNS peers, and (3) more behaviorally inhibited CWS, when compared to less behaviorally inhibited CWS, exhibited more stuttering.

Conclusions: Findings are taken to suggest that one aspect of temperament (i.e., behavioral inhibition) is exhibited by some preschool-age CWS and that these children stutter more than CWS with lower behavioral inhibition. The present results seem to support continued study of the association between young children's temperamental characteristics and stuttering, the diagnostic entity (i.e., CWS versus CWNS), as well as stuttering, the behavior (e.g., frequency of stuttered disfluencies).

Educational objectives: After reading this article, the reader will be able to: (a) summarize the salient empirical findings in the extant literature with regard to the association between temperament and childhood stuttering; (b) describe the concept of behavioral inhibition (BI) as well as the methods to measure BI; and (c) discuss the association between behavioral inhibition and childhood stuttering in preschool-age children.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Spearman rank-ordercorrelation between latency to the 6 th spontaneous comment and frequency of stuttered disfluencies per 100 words for preschool-age CWS (N=26).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Anderson J, Pellowski M, Conture E, Kelly E. Temperamental characteristics of young children who stutter. Journal of Speech Language, and Hearing Research. 2003;46(5):1221–1233. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Arnold H, Conture E, Key A, Walden T. Emotional reactivity, regulation and childhood stuttering: A behavioral and electrophysiological study. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2011;44(3):276–293. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Blanchard D, Blanchard R. Ethoexperimental approaches to the biology of emotion. In: Rosenzweig MR, Porter LW, editors. Annual Reviews (Palo Alto, CA) Vol. 39. 1988. pp. 43–68. Annual review of psychology. - PubMed
    1. Bloodstein O. A handbook on stuttering. 5 . San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc; 1995.
    1. Brown R. A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1973.

Publication types