Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2013 Oct 1;305(7):R840-53.
doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00297.2013. Epub 2013 Aug 7.

Post-oral appetite stimulation by sugars and nonmetabolizable sugar analogs

Affiliations
Comparative Study

Post-oral appetite stimulation by sugars and nonmetabolizable sugar analogs

Steven Zukerman et al. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. .

Abstract

Post-oral sugar actions enhance the intake of and preference for sugar-rich foods, a process referred to as appetition. Here, we investigated the role of intestinal sodium glucose cotransporters (SGLTs) in sugar appetition in C57BL/6J mice using sugars and nonmetabolizable sugar analogs that differ in their affinity for SGLT1 and SGLT3. In experiments 1 and 2, food-restricted mice were trained (1 h/day) to consume a flavored saccharin solution [conditioned stimulus (CS-)] paired with intragastric (IG) self-infusions of water and a different flavored solution (CS+) paired with infusions of 8 or 12% sugars (glucose, fructose, and galactose) or sugar analogs (α-methyl-D-glucopyranoside, MDG; 3-O-methyl-D-glucopyranoside, OMG). Subsequent two-bottle CS+ vs. CS- choice tests were conducted without coinfusions. Infusions of the SGLT1 ligands glucose, galactose, MDG, and OMG stimulated CS+ licking above CS- levels. However, only glucose, MDG, and galactose conditioned significant CS+ preferences, with the SGLT3 ligands (glucose, MDG) producing the strongest preferences. Fructose, which is not a ligand for SGLTs, failed to stimulate CS+ intake or preference. Experiment 3 revealed that IG infusion of MDG+phloridzin (an SGLT1/3 antagonist) blocked MDG appetition, whereas phloridzin had minimal effects on glucose-induced appetition. However, adding phloretin (a GLUT2 antagonist) to the glucose+phloridzin infusion blocked glucose appetition. Taken together, these findings suggest that humoral signals generated by intestinal SGLT1 and SGLT3, and to a lesser degree, GLUT2, mediate post-oral sugar appetition in mice. The MDG results indicate that sugar metabolism is not essential for the post-oral intake-stimulating and preference-conditioning actions of sugars in mice.

Keywords: fructose; galactose; glucose; post-oral sugar conditioning.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Experiment 1A: Values are expressed as means ± SE. A: 1-h total licks are plotted for one bottle tests 0–3. The mice drank (1 h/day) a conditioned stimuli (CS)-flavored saccharin solution paired with intragastric (IG) water infusions in test 0 before being switched to a CS+-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG sugar self-infusions in tests 1–3. The 8% GLU, 8% FRU, and 8% GAL groups were infused with 8% glucose, fructose, and galactose, respectively. B: 1-h licks are plotted for CS+- and CS−-flavored saccharin solutions during the two-bottle preference test for the 8% GLU, 8% FRU, and 8% GAL groups. CS+ and CS− intakes were not paired with IG infusions during test. Number atop bar represents mean percent preference for the CS+ solution. *Significant difference (P < 0.05) between test 0 vs. tests 1–3 licks and between CS+ vs. CS− licks.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Experiment 1B: Values are expressed as means ± SE. A: 1-h total licks are plotted for one bottle tests 0–3. The mice drank (1 h/day) a CS−-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG water infusions in test 0 before being switched to a CS+-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG sugar self-infusions in tests 1–3. The 12% GLU, 12% FRU, and 12% GAL groups were infused with 12% glucose, fructose, and galactose, respectively. B: 1-h licks are plotted for CS+- and CS−-flavored saccharin solutions during the two-bottle preference test for the 12% GLU, 12% FRU, and 12% GAL groups. CS+ and CS− intakes were not paired with IG infusions during the test. Number atop bar represents mean percent preference for the CS+ solution. *Significant differences (P < 0.05) between test 0 vs. tests 1–3 licks and between CS+ vs. CS− licks.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Experiment 2A: Values are expressed as means ± SE. A: 1-h total licks are plotted for one bottle tests 0–3. The mice drank (1 h/day) a CS−-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG water infusions in test 0 before being switched to a CS+-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG sugar self-infusions in tests 1–3. The 8% GLU, 8% MDG, and 8% OMG groups were infused with 8% glucose, MDG and OMG, respectively. B: 1-h licks are plotted for CS+- and CS−-flavored saccharin solutions during the two-bottle preference test for the 8% GLU, MDG, and OMG groups. CS+ and CS− intakes were not paired with IG infusions during the test. Number atop bar represents mean percent preference for the CS+ solution. *Significant differences (P < 0.05) between test 0 vs. tests 1–3 licks and between CS+ vs. CS− licks.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
Experiment 2B: Values are expressed as means ± SE. A: 1-h total licks are plotted for one bottle tests 0–3. The mice drank (1 h/day) a CS−-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG water infusions in test 0 before being switched to a CS+-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG sugar self-infusions in tests 1–3. The 12% GLU, 12% MDG, and 12% OMG groups were infused with 12% glucose, MDG, and OMG, respectively. B: 1-h licks are plotted for CS+- and CS−-flavored saccharin solutions during the two-bottle preference test for the 12% GLU, MDG, and OMG groups. CS+ and CS− intakes were not paired with IG infusions during the test. Number atop bar represents mean percent preference for the CS+ solution. *Significant differences (P < 0.05) between test 0 vs. tests 1–3 licks and between CS+ vs. CS− licks.
Fig. 5.
Fig. 5.
Experiment 2: Values are expressed as means ± SE. A: blood glucose at 0, 15, 30, and 60 min after a 0.6-ml infusion of glucose, MDG, or OMG in 8% and 12% groups or water in the H2O group. B: incremental area under the curve (IAUC) for blood glucose after a 0.6-ml infusion of glucose, MDG, or OMG in 8% and 12% groups or water in the H2O group.
Fig. 6.
Fig. 6.
Experiment 3A: Values are expressed as means ± SE. A: 1-h total licks are plotted for one bottle tests 0–3. The mice drank (1 h/day) a CS-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG water infusions in test 0 before being switched to a CS+-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG sugar infusions. The MDG+phloridzin (Pz), MDG, GLU+Pz, and GLU groups were infused with 8% MDG, 8% MDG+0.4% Pz, 8% glucose, and 8% glucose+ 0.4% Pz, respectively. B: 1-h licks are plotted for CS+ and CS−-flavored saccharin solutions during the two-bottle preference test for the MDG, MDG+Pz, GLU, and GLU+Pz groups. CS+ and CS− intakes were not paired with IG infusions during the test. Number atop bar represents mean percent preference for the CS+ solution. *Significant differences (P < 0.05) between test 0 vs. tests 1–3 licks and between CS+ vs. CS− licks.
Fig. 7.
Fig. 7.
Experiment 3A: Values are expressed as means ± SE. A: blood glucose at 0, 15, 30, and 60 min after a 0.6-ml infusion of 8% glucose, 8% glucose+0.4% phloridzin, or water in the GLU, GLU+Pz, and H2O groups. B: blood glucose IAUC in the GLU, GLU+Pz, and H2O groups.
Fig. 8.
Fig. 8.
Experiment 3B: Values are expressed as means ± SE. A: 1-h total licks are plotted for one bottle tests 0–3. The mice drank (1 h/day) a CS−-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG water infusions in test 0 before being switched to a CS+-flavored saccharin solution paired with IG sugar infusions. The GLU+Pz+phloretin (Pt) and GLU+Pz groups were infused with 8% glucose+0.4% Pz+0.23% Pt and 8% glucose+0.4% Pz containing 3.25% 1 N NaOH, respectively. B: 1-h licks are plotted for CS+- and CS−-flavored saccharin solutions during the two-bottle preference test with the GLU+Pz+Pt and GLU+Pz groups. CS+ and CS− intakes were not paired with IG infusions during the test. Number atop bar represents mean percent preference for the CS+ solution. *Significant differences (P < 0.05) between test 0 vs. tests 1–3 licks and between CS+ vs. CS− licks.
Fig. 9.
Fig. 9.
Experiment 3B: Values are expressed as means ± SE. A: blood glucose at 0, 15, 30, and 60 min after a 0.6-ml infusion of 8% glucose+0.4% Pz, 8% glucose+0.4% Pz+0.23% Pt, and water in the GLU+Pz, GLU+Pz+Pt, and H2O groups, respectively. All infusions contained 3.25% of 1 N NaOH. B: blood glucose IAUC in the GLU+Pz, GLU+Pz+Pt, and H2O groups.

References

    1. Ackroff K, Sclafani A, Axen KV. Diabetic rats prefer glucose-paired flavors over fructose-paired flavors. Appetite 28: 73–83, 1997 - PubMed
    1. Ackroff K, Yiin YM, Sclafani A. Post-oral infusion sites that support glucose-conditioned flavor preferences in rats. Physiol Behav 99: 402–411, 2010 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Aljure O, Díez-Sampedro A. Functional characterization of mouse sodium/glucose transporter type 3b. Am J Physiol Cell Physiol 299: C58–C65, 2010 - PubMed
    1. Berman WF, Bautista JO, Rogers S, Segal S. Metabolism and transport of galactose by rat intestine. Biochim Biophys Acta 455: 90–101, 1976 - PubMed
    1. Berthoud HR. Vagal and hormonal gut-brain communication: from satiation to satisfaction. Neurogastroenterol Motil 20: 64–72, 2008 - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

MeSH terms