No dedicated second-order motion system
- PMID: 24013864
- DOI: 10.1167/13.11.2
No dedicated second-order motion system
Abstract
The existence of a second-order motion system distinct from both the first-order and feature tracking motion systems remains controversial even though many consider it well established. In the present study, the texture contribution to motion was measured within and beyond the spatial acuity of attention by presenting the stimuli in the near periphery where the spatial resolution of attention is low. The logic was that when moving elements are too close one to another for attention to individually select them (i.e., crowding), it is not possible to track them. To test the existence of a dedicated second-order motion system, the texture contribution to motion was measured when neutralizing both the feature tracking motion system and the contribution of the first-order motion system due to preprocessing nonlinearities introducing residual distortion products. When the contribution of distortion products was not neutralized, texture substantially contributed to motion for spatial frequencies within and beyond the spatial acuity of attention. When neutralizing the contribution of distortion products, texture substantially contributed to motion for spatial frequencies within the spatial acuity of attention, but not for spatial frequencies beyond the spatial acuity of attention. We conclude that there is no dedicated second-order motion system; the texture contribution to motion is mediated solely by the first-order (due to residual distortion products) and feature tracking (at frequencies within spatiotemporal acuity of attention) motion systems.
Keywords: feature-tracking; motion; residual distortion products; second-order.
Similar articles
-
No second-order motion system sensitive to high temporal frequencies.J Vis. 2013 Apr 4;13(5):4. doi: 10.1167/13.5.4. J Vis. 2013. PMID: 23559594
-
Second-order motion discrimination by feature-tracking.Vision Res. 1999 Apr;39(8):1465-75. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00227-2. Vision Res. 1999. PMID: 10343815
-
Sensitivity to spatial and temporal modulations of first-order and second-order motion.Vision Res. 2006 Feb;46(3):324-35. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.03.002. Epub 2005 Apr 20. Vision Res. 2006. PMID: 16360001
-
Central neural mechanisms for detecting second-order motion.Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999 Aug;9(4):461-6. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80069-5. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999. PMID: 10448168 Review.
-
Full-wave and half-wave processes in second-order motion and texture.Ciba Found Symp. 1994;184:287-303; discussion 303-8, 330-8. doi: 10.1002/9780470514610.ch15. Ciba Found Symp. 1994. PMID: 7882759 Review.
Cited by
-
The Role of Feature Tracking in the Furrow Illusion.Front Hum Neurosci. 2016 Mar 7;10:81. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00081. eCollection 2016. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016. PMID: 27014018 Free PMC article.
-
Broad bandwidth of perceptual learning in second-order contrast modulation detection.J Vis. 2015 Feb 16;15(2):20. doi: 10.1167/15.2.20. J Vis. 2015. PMID: 25686623 Free PMC article.
-
Orientation-cue invariant population responses to contrast-modulated and phase-reversed contour stimuli in macaque V1 and V2.PLoS One. 2014 Sep 4;9(9):e106753. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106753. eCollection 2014. PLoS One. 2014. PMID: 25188576 Free PMC article.
-
Overcoming the limitations of motion sensor models by considering dendritic computations.Sci Rep. 2025 Mar 17;15(1):9213. doi: 10.1038/s41598-025-90095-z. Sci Rep. 2025. PMID: 40097493 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources