Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Feb;40(1):361-77.
doi: 10.1037/a0034394. Epub 2013 Oct 7.

Angular declination and the dynamic perception of egocentric distance

Affiliations

Angular declination and the dynamic perception of egocentric distance

Daniel A Gajewski et al. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2014 Feb.

Abstract

The extraction of the distance between an object and an observer is fast when angular declination is informative, as it is with targets placed on the ground. To what extent does angular declination drive performance when viewing time is limited? Participants judged target distances in a real-world environment with viewing durations ranging from 36-220 ms. An important role for angular declination was supported by experiments showing that the cue provides information about egocentric distance even on the very first glimpse, and that it supports a sensitive response to distance in the absence of other useful cues. Performance was better at 220-ms viewing durations than for briefer glimpses, suggesting that the perception of distance is dynamic even within the time frame of a typical eye fixation. Critically, performance in limited viewing trials was better when preceded by a 15-s preview of the room without a designated target. The results indicate that the perception of distance is powerfully shaped by memory from prior visual experience with the scene. A theoretical framework for the dynamic perception of distance is presented.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
The distance to a target object resting on the ground is trigonometrically related to angular declination below eye level but depends on the perception and/or representation of surface slant.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean walked distance to floor-level targets as a function of target distance and the physical size of object in Experiment 1. The targets were of constant size or were varied to hold angular size constant. Size was blocked such that either the varied-size trials were administered first (left) or the constant-size trials were administered first (right). Figures include best-fitting regression lines through the average data. Depicted are the means with standard error bars and best-fitting lines for the fixed effects derived from the model.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean walked distance to floor-level targets as a function of target distance and trial number in Experiment 2. The viewing duration was 74 ms. Figure includes best-fitting regression lines through the average data. Depicted are the means with standard error bars and best-fitting lines for the fixed effects derived from the model.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Mean walked distance to floor-level targets as a function of target distance and the viewing condition in Experiment 3. The illuminated targets were monocularly viewed in a darkened room (Dark) for 3 seconds and standard targets were viewed binocularly in a well lit room (Light) for 74 ms. Viewing condition was blocked such that either the Dark trials were administered first (left) or the Light trials were administered first (right). Depicted are the means with standard error bars and best-fitting lines for the fixed effects derived from the model.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Mean walked distance to floor-level targets as a function of target distance and the viewing condition in Experiment 4. Standard targets were at each participant’s brief (Suprathreshold) and long (220 ms) viewing duration. Viewing duration was blocked such that either the Suprathreshold trials were administered first (left) or the 220-ms trials were administered first (right). Depicted are the means with standard error bars and best-fitting lines for the fixed effects derived from the model.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Mean walked distance to floor-level targets as a function of target distance in Experiment 5. A block of blind walking trials with a viewing duration of 74 ms was preceded by a 15-second visual preview of the room (Visual Preview), a set of directed-walking trials (Walking Preview), or no-preview control (None). Depicted are the means with standard error bars and best-fitting lines for the fixed effects derived from the model.

References

    1. Adam JJ, Davelaar EJ, van der Gou A, Willems P. Evidence for attentional processing in spatial localization. Psychological Research. 2008;72:433–442. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Adam JJ, Paas FGWC, Ekering J, van Loon EM. Spatial localization: Tests of a two-process model. Experimental Brain Research. 1995;102:531–539. - PubMed
    1. Allison RS, Gillam BJ, Vescillio E. Binocular depth discrimination and estimation beyond interaction space. Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):10, 1–14. - PubMed
    1. Avraamides MN, Loomis JM, Klatzky RL, Golledge RG. Functional equivalence of spatial representations derived from vision and language: Evidence from allocentric judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2004;30:801–814. - PubMed
    1. Barbot A, Landy MS, Carrasco M. Differential effects of exogenous and endogenous attention on second-order texture contrast sensitivity. Journal of Vision. 2012;12(8):6, 1–15. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types