Navigating in a three-dimensional world
- PMID: 24103594
- DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X12002476
Navigating in a three-dimensional world
Abstract
The study of spatial cognition has provided considerable insight into how animals (including humans) navigate on the horizontal plane. However, the real world is three-dimensional, having a complex topography including both horizontal and vertical features, which presents additional challenges for representation and navigation. The present article reviews the emerging behavioral and neurobiological literature on spatial cognition in non-horizontal environments. We suggest that three-dimensional spaces are represented in a quasi-planar fashion, with space in the plane of locomotion being computed separately and represented differently from space in the orthogonal axis - a representational structure we have termed "bicoded." We argue that the mammalian spatial representation in surface-travelling animals comprises a mosaic of these locally planar fragments, rather than a fully integrated volumetric map. More generally, this may be true even for species that can move freely in all three dimensions, such as birds and fish. We outline the evidence supporting this view, together with the adaptive advantages of such a scheme.
Comment in
-
Semantic sides of three-dimensional space representation.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):543; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000307. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103595
-
Human path navigation in a three-dimensional world.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):544-5; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000319. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103596
-
Learning landmarks and routes in multi-floored buildings.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):545; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000320. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103597
-
Anisotropy and polarization of space: evidence from naïve optics and phenomenological psychophysics.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):545-6; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000332. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103598
-
Navigating in a volumetric world: metric encoding in the vertical axis of space.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):546-7; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000344. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103599
-
Think local, act global: how do fragmented representations of space allow seamless navigation?Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):548-9; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000368. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103601
-
Perceptual experience as a bridge between the retina and a bicoded cognitive map.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):549; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X1300037X. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103602
-
Learning to navigate in a three-dimensional world: from bees to primates.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):550; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000381. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103603
-
Spatial language as a window on representations of three-dimensional space.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):550-1; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000393. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103604
-
Multi-floor buildings and human wayfinding cognition.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):551-2; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X1300040X. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103605
-
Applying the bicoded spatial model to nonhuman primates in an arboreal multilayer environment.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):552-3; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000411. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103606
-
The complex interplay between three-dimensional egocentric and allocentric spatial representation.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):553-4; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000423. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103607
-
The planar mosaic fails to account for spatially directed action.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):554-5; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000435. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103608
-
Monkeys in space: primate neural data suggest volumetric representations.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):555-6; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000447. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103609
-
Development of human spatial cognition in a three-dimensional world.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):556; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000459. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103610
-
Has a fully three-dimensional space map never evolved in any species? A comparative imperative for studies of spatial cognition.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):557; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000460. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103611
-
Making a stronger case for comparative research to investigate the behavioral and neurological bases of three-dimensional navigation.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):557-8; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000472. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103612
-
Which animal model for understanding human navigation in a three-dimensional world?Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):558-9; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000484. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103613
-
The study of blindness and technology can reveal the mechanisms of three-dimensional navigation.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):559-60; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000496. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103614
-
Augmented topological maps for three-dimensional navigation.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):560-1; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000502. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103615
-
Navigation bicoded as functions of x-y and time?Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):561-2; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000514. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103616
-
Vertical and veridical--2.5-dimensional visual and vestibular navigation.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):562-3; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000526. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103617
-
Foreshortening affects both uphill and downhill slope perception at far distances.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):563-4; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000538. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103618
-
The problem of conflicting reference frames when investigating three-dimensional space in surface-dwelling animals.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):564-5; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X1300054X. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103619
-
Just the tip of the iceberg: the bicoded map is but one instantiation of scalable spatial representation structures.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):565-6; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000551. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103620
-
What is optimized in an optimal path?Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):566; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000563. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103621
-
Grid maps for spaceflight, anyone? They are for free!Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):566-7; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000575. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103622
-
What counts as the evidence for three-dimensional and four-dimensional spatial representations?Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):567-8; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000587. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103623
-
Are all types of vertical information created equal?Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):568-9; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000599. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103624
-
Map fragmentation in two- and three-dimensional environments.Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):569-71; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000605. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103625
-
Does evidence from ethology support bicoded cognitive maps?Behav Brain Sci. 2013 Oct;36(5):570-1; discussion 571-87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000617. Behav Brain Sci. 2013. PMID: 24103626
Publication types
MeSH terms
Grants and funding
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources