Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Randomized Controlled Trial
. 2013 Dec;21(6):499-506.
doi: 10.1037/a0034575. Epub 2013 Oct 14.

Attentional bias for nondrug reward is magnified in addiction

Affiliations
Randomized Controlled Trial

Attentional bias for nondrug reward is magnified in addiction

Brian A Anderson et al. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013 Dec.

Abstract

Attentional biases for drug-related stimuli play a prominent role in addiction, predicting treatment outcomes. Attentional biases also develop for stimuli that have been paired with nondrug rewards in adults without a history of addiction, the magnitude of which is predicted by visual working-memory capacity and impulsiveness. We tested the hypothesis that addiction is associated with an increased attentional bias for nondrug (monetary) reward relative to that of healthy controls, and that this bias is related to working-memory impairments and increased impulsiveness. Seventeen patients receiving methadone-maintenance treatment for opioid dependence and 17 healthy controls participated. Impulsiveness was measured using the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995), visual working-memory capacity was measured as the ability to recognize briefly presented color stimuli, and attentional bias was measured as the magnitude of response time slowing caused by irrelevant but previously reward-associated distractors in a visual-search task. The results showed that attention was biased toward the distractors across all participants, replicating previous findings. It is important to note, this bias was significantly greater in the patients than in the controls and was negatively correlated with visual working-memory capacity. Patients were also significantly more impulsive than controls as a group. Our findings demonstrate that patients in treatment for addiction experience greater difficulty ignoring stimuli associated with nondrug reward. This nonspecific reward-related bias could mediate the distracting quality of drug-related stimuli previously observed in addiction.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Disclosures

All authors made substantive contributions and have read and approved the final manuscript.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Sequence of events and time course for a trial during the training phase (A) and test phase (B) of the visual search task. During the training phase, patients and controls searched for a target circle that was unpredictably red or green, and received a monetary reward for correctly reporting the orientation of a bar contained within the target. During the test phase, participants searched for a target defined as the unique shape (e.g., diamond among circles) and no monetary rewards were provided. On a subset of the trials, one of the non-target shapes was rendered in the color of a formerly reward-predictive target (i.e., red or green), which served as the reward-associated distractor.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Response time across the three distractor conditions in the test phase for patients and controls. There was a main effect of distractor condition in which participants were significantly slower to report the target when a high-value distractor was present compared to when no distractor was present (“absent”), indicating value-driven attentional capture (**p < .001). The magnitude of this slowing was significantly greater in the patients than in the controls (*p < .01), indicating greater susceptibility to attentional capture by previously reward-associated stimuli. Error bars reflect the within-subjects standard error of the mean.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Correlation between value-driven attentional capture (slowing in RT caused by the high-value distractor) and visual working memory capacity separately for patients (r = −.697, p = .002) and controls (r = −.046, p = .860).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Anderson BA. A value-driven mechanism of attentional selection. Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):7, 1–16. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S. Reward predictions bias attentional selection. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:262. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S. Generalization of value-based attentional priority. Visual Cognition. 2012;20:647–658. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S. Learned value magnifies salience-based attentional capture. PLoS ONE. 2011a;6:e27926. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S. Value-driven attentional capture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America. 2011b;108:10367–10371. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types