Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Sep 26:7:618.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00618. eCollection 2013.

Flexible spatial perspective-taking: conversational partners weigh multiple cues in collaborative tasks

Affiliations

Flexible spatial perspective-taking: conversational partners weigh multiple cues in collaborative tasks

Alexia Galati et al. Front Hum Neurosci. .

Abstract

Research on spatial perspective-taking often focuses on the cognitive processes of isolated individuals as they adopt or maintain imagined perspectives. Collaborative studies of spatial perspective-taking typically examine speakers' linguistic choices, while overlooking their underlying processes and representations. We review evidence from two collaborative experiments that examine the contribution of social and representational cues to spatial perspective choices in both language and the organization of spatial memory. Across experiments, speakers organized their memory representations according to the convergence of various cues. When layouts were randomly configured and did not afford intrinsic cues, speakers encoded their partner's viewpoint in memory, if available, but did not use it as an organizing direction. On the other hand, when the layout afforded an intrinsic structure, speakers organized their spatial memories according to the person-centered perspective reinforced by the layout's structure. Similarly, in descriptions, speakers considered multiple cues whether available a priori or at the interaction. They used partner-centered expressions more frequently (e.g., "to your right") when the partner's viewpoint was misaligned by a small offset or coincided with the layout's structure. Conversely, they used egocentric expressions more frequently when their own viewpoint coincided with the intrinsic structure or when the partner was misaligned by a computationally difficult, oblique offset. Based on these findings we advocate for a framework for flexible perspective-taking: people weigh multiple cues (including social ones) to make attributions about the relative difficulty of perspective-taking for each partner, and adapt behavior to minimize their collective effort. This framework is not specialized for spatial reasoning but instead emerges from the same principles and memory-depended processes that govern perspective-taking in non-spatial tasks.

Keywords: audience design; common ground; intrinsic structure; perspective-taking; spatial descriptions; spatial memory.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
One of the three seven-object layouts used in Galati et al. (2013),whose configuration was designed to appear seemingly random. It comprised a battery, a flashlight, a bowl, an orange, a yoyo, a button, and a vase. The arrows represent the Director's viewpoint (0°), and the Matcher's viewpoint when offset by 90°, 135°, and 180°.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Set-up of our studies, showing the Director's and Matcher's working stations, and the locations of recording devices. This example of a description phase illustrates the conditions of Galati et al. (2013) in which Directors and Matchers were misaligned by 135°, and the conditions of Galati and Avraamides (in revision) in which Directors were aligned with the intrinsic structure (with Matchers misaligned by 135°).
Figure 3
Figure 3
The seven-object layout used in Galati and Avraamides (in revision), whose configuration had an intrinsic structure. It comprised a flashlight, a yoyo, a bucket, a battery, a candle, a marble, and a vase. The arrows at 0°, 135°, and 225° represent the viewpoint that Directors and Matchers occupied at the different conditions of their relative alignment with the intrinsic structure.

References

    1. Avraamides M. N., Theodorou M., Agathokleous A., Nicolaou A. (2013). Revisiting Perspective-taking: can people maintain imagined perspectives? Spat. Cogn. Comput. 13, 50–78 10.1080/13875868.2011.639915 - DOI
    1. Bortfeld H., Brennan S. E. (1997). Use and acquisition of idiomatic expressions in referring by native and non-native speakers. Discourse Process. 23, 119–147 10.1080/01638537709544986 - DOI
    1. Brennan S. E. (2005). How conversation is shaped by visual and spoken evidence, in Approaches to Studying World-Situated Language Use: Bridging the Language-as-Product and Language-Action Traditions, eds Trueswell J. C., Tanenhaus M. K. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; ), 95–129
    1. Brennan S. E., Hanna J. E. (2009). Partner-specific adaptation in dialog. Top. Cogn. Sci. 1, 274–291 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01019.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brennan S. E., Galati A., Kuhlen A. K. (2010). Two minds, one dialog: coordinating speaking and understanding, in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 53, ed Ross B. H. (Burlington, MA: Academic Press/Elsevier; ), 301–345 10.1016/S0079-7421(10)53008-1 - DOI

LinkOut - more resources