Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Nov 13;281(1774):20132531.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2531. Print 2014 Jan 7.

Social status and personality: stability in social state can promote consistency of behavioural responses

Affiliations

Social status and personality: stability in social state can promote consistency of behavioural responses

Anna Favati et al. Proc Biol Sci. .

Abstract

Stability of 'state' has been suggested as an underlying factor explaining behavioural stability and animal personality (i.e. variation among, and consistency within individuals in behavioural responses), but the possibility that stable social relationships represent such states remains unexplored. Here, we investigated the influence of social status on the expression and consistency of behaviours by experimentally changing social status between repeated personality assays. We used male domestic fowl (Gallus gallus domesticus), a social species that forms relatively stable dominance hierarchies, and showed that behavioural responses were strongly affected by social status, but also by individual characteristics. The level of vigilance, activity and exploration changed with social status, whereas boldness appeared as a stable individual property, independent of status. Furthermore, variation in vocalization predicted future social status, indicating that individual behaviours can both be a predictor and a consequence of social status, depending on the aspect in focus. Our results illustrate that social states contribute to both variation and stability in behavioural responses, and should therefore be taken into account when investigating and interpreting variation in personality.

Keywords: behavioural syndromes; chicken; intra-sexual selection; phenotypic plasticity; social dominance.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Behavioural response of dominant and subordinate males in the first trial of a novel arena test. Dominant males (‘dom’, filled circles) (a) tended to be bolder (i.e. more often entered the arena within 30 s), (b) were more vigilant (i.e. had higher frequency of time spent being vigilant), (c) were more active (i.e. performed more subarea transitions), (d) tended to be more explorative (i.e. visited more subareas), and (e) crowed more often (i.e. number of crows) compared with subordinate males (‘sub’, open circles; table 1). Values are given as untransformed mean±95% confidence interval (CI), except for (a), where the proportion of bold males±95% CI, are given.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
The effect of current social status (in trial 2) and previous behavioural responses (in the first novel arena test, i.e. trial 1’) on the responses of male fowl in a novel arena test after social status was manipulated (i.e. ‘trial 2’). (a) Status in trial 2 (dominant males: filled circles, subdominant males: open circles) did not affect the proportion of males that were bold (i.e. males that entered the arena within 30 s), but previous boldness (x-axis) had a significant effect on boldness in trial 2 (y-axis). Males that were dominant in trial 2 (filled circles, solid line) were (b) more vigilant (i.e. had a higher frequency of time spent being vigilant), (c) more active (i.e. conducted more subarea transitions), and (d) more explorative (i.e. visited more subareas) compared with males that were subdominant (open circles, dashed line). At the same time, these behaviours were repeatable within individuals (demonstrated by a positive slope; table 2). (e) Social status in trial 2 did not affect crowing. The figures show transformed response variables (for transformations, see main text).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Vigilance of male fowl of different social status during the first (trial 1) and second test trial (trial 2) in a repeated novel arena test. Dominant males (filled circles) were more vigilant (i.e. frequency of time spent being vigilant) compared with subordinate males (open circles). Current social status affected this behaviour, such that males who changed social status also changed the time spent being vigilant (dashed lines), whereas males with the same status during both trials were more consistent in their frequency of vigilance. Values presented are mean ± 95% CI. See the electronic supplementary material for similar figures for other behaviours (figure S2).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Mitchell-Olds T, Willis JH, Goldstein DB. 2007. Which evolutionary processes influence natural genetic variation for phenotypic traits? Nat. Rev. Genet. 8, 845–856 (doi:10.1038/nrg2207) - DOI - PubMed
    1. Dall SRX, Houston AI, McNamara JM. 2004. The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecol. Lett. 7, 734–739 (doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00618.x) - DOI
    1. Reale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ. 2007. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol. Rev. 82, 291–318 (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x) - DOI - PubMed
    1. Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, Van Der Vegt BJ, Van Reenen CG, Hopster H, De Jong IC, Ruis MAW, Blokhuis HJ. 1999. Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 925–935 (doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00026-3) - DOI - PubMed
    1. Gosling SD. 2001. From mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research? Psychol. Bull. 127, 45–86 (doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.45) - DOI - PubMed

Publication types