Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Dec 3:4:868.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00868. eCollection 2013.

Social learning from humans or conspecifics: differences and similarities between wolves and dogs

Affiliations

Social learning from humans or conspecifics: differences and similarities between wolves and dogs

Friederike Range et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Most domestication hypotheses propose that dogs have been selected for enhanced communication and interactions with humans, including learning socially from human demonstrators. However, to what extent these skills are newly derived and to what extent they originate from wolf-wolf interactions is unclear. In order to test for the possible origins of dog social cognition, we need to compare the interactions of wolves and dogs with humans and with conspecifics. Here, we tested identically raised and kept juvenile wolves and dogs in a social learning task with human and conspecific demonstrators. Using a local enhancement task, we found that both wolves and dogs benefitted from a demonstration independent of the demonstrator species in comparison to a control, no demonstration condition. Interestingly, if the demonstrator only pretended to hide food at the target location, wolves and dogs reacted differently: while dogs differentiated between this without-food and with-food demonstration independent of the demonstrator species, wolves only did so in case of human demonstrators. We attribute this finding to wolves being more attentive toward behavioral details of the conspecific models than the dogs: although the demonstrator dogs were trained to execute the demonstration, they disliked the food reward, which might have decreased the interest of the wolves in finding the food reward. Overall, these results suggest that dogs but also wolves can use information provided by both human and conspecific demonstrators in a local enhancement task. Therefore we suggest that a more fine-scale analysis of dog and wolf social learning is needed to determine the effects of domestication.

Keywords: conspecific demonstrator; dog; domestication; human demonstrator; local enhancement; wolf.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Experimental set-up.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Proportion of success finding the chick for wolves and dogs in the dog demonstrator (DOG_DEM), human demonstrator (HUMAN_DEM) and smell control (SMELL_CON) condition.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Box plots showing the successful wolves and dogs’ latency to find the chick in the 3 conditions. Boxes represent the interquartile range, bars within shaded boxes are median values, whiskers indicate the 5th and 95th percentiles, and open circles are outliers.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
The graph presents the percentage of success of all animals (wolves and dogs combined) at a certain age in the human demonstration (HUMAN_DEM), dog demonstration (DOG_DEM) and smell control (SMELL_CON) condition.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
The success of dogs (A) and wolves (B) in the dog and human demonstration (DOG_DEM and HUMAN_DEM) and control conditions (DOG_CON and HUMAN_CON).
FIGURE 6
FIGURE 6
Box plots showing the relative duration that the wolves and dogs observed the various demonstrations. Boxes represent the interquartile range, bars within shaded boxes are median values, whiskers indicate the 5th and 95th percentiles, and open circles are outliers.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Asa C. S., Mech D. (1995). “A review of the sensory organs in wolves and their importance to life history,” in Ecology and Conservation of Wolves in a Changing World eds. Carbyn L. D., Fritts S. H., Seip D. R. (Edmonton: Canadian Circumpolar Institute; ) 287–291.
    1. Boitani L., Ciucci P. (1995). Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 7 49–72. 10.1080/08927014.1995.9522969 - DOI
    1. Bonanni R., Cafazzo S., Valsecchi P., Natoli E. (2010a). Effect of group size, dominance rank and social bonding on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Anim. Behav. 79 981–991. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.021 - DOI
    1. Bonanni R., Valsecchi P., Natoli E. (2010b). Pattern of individual participation and cheating in conflicts between groups of free-ranging dogs. Anim. Behav. 79 957–968. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.016 - DOI
    1. Butler J. R. A., Du Toit J. T., Bingham J. (2004). Free-ranging domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) as predators and prey in rural Zimbabwe: threats of competition and disease to large wild carnivores. Biol. Conserv. 115 369–378. 10.1016/S0006-3207(03)00152-6 - DOI

LinkOut - more resources