Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Jun 4:5:539.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00539. eCollection 2014.

Conscious and unconscious context-specific cognitive control

Affiliations

Conscious and unconscious context-specific cognitive control

Nathalie Schouppe et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

A key feature of the human cognitive system is its ability to deal with an ever-changing environment. One prototypical example is the observation that we adjust our information processing depending on the conflict-likelihood of a context (context-specific proportion congruency effect, CSPC, Crump etal., 2006). Recently, empirical studies started to question the role of consciousness in these strategic adaptation processes (for reviews, see Desender and Van den Bussche, 2012; Kunde etal., 2012). However, these studies have not yielded unequivocal results (e.g., Kunde, 2003; Heinemann etal., 2009; Van Gaal etal., 2010a; Desender etal., 2013; Reuss etal., 2014). In the present study, we aim at replicating the experiment of Heinemann etal. (2009) in which the proportion of congruent and incongruent trials between different contexts was varied in a masked priming task. Their results showed a reduction of the congruency effect for the context with more incongruent trials. However, this CSPC effect was only observed when the prime-target conflict was conscious, rather than unconscious, suggesting that context-specific control operates within the boundaries of awareness. Our replication attempt however contrasts these findings. In the first experiment we found no evidence for a CSPC effect in reaction times (RTs), neither in the conscious nor in the unconscious condition. The error rate analysis did show a CSPC effect, albeit not one modulated by consciousness. In the second experiment we found an overall CSPC effect in RTs, independent of consciousness. The error rates did not display a CSPC pattern. These mixed results seem to nuance the findings of Heinemann etal. (2009) and highlight the need for replication studies in psychology research.

Keywords: CSPC effect; cognitive control; consciousness; context; masked priming.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
(A) Mean RTs for each condition, depicting the CSPC effect (calculated on the test trials only) in each session, separately for the conscious and unconscious group and (B) mean error rates for each condition, depicting the CSPC effect (calculated on the test trials only) in each session, separately for the conscious and unconscious group.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
(A) Mean RTs for each condition, depicting the CSPC effect (calculated on the test trials only) in each session, separately for the conscious and unconscious group and (B) mean error rates for each condition, depicting the CSPC effect (calculated on the test trials only) in each session, separately for the conscious and unconscious group.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ansorge U., Fuchs I., Khalid S., Kunde W. (2011). No conflict control in the absence of awareness. Psychol. Res. 75 351–365 10.1007/s00426-010-0313-4 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Botvinick M. M., Braver T. S., Barch D. M., Carter C. S., Cohen J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108 624–652 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bugg J. M, Crump M. J. C. (2012). In support of a distinction between voluntary and stimulus-driven control: a review of the literature on proportion congruent effects. Front. Psychol. 3:367. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Crump M. J., Milliken B. (2009). The flexibility of context-specific control: evidence for context-driven generalization of item-specific control settings. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 62 1523–1532 10.1080/17470210902752096 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Crump M. J. C., Gong Z., Milliken B. (2006). The context-specific proportion congruent Stroop effect: location as a contextual cue. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13 316–321 10.3758/BF03193850 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources