Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Jul:45:46-59.
doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.008.

Nature, Nurture, and Expertise

Affiliations

Nature, Nurture, and Expertise

Robert Plomin et al. Intelligence. 2014 Jul.

Abstract

Rather than investigating the extent to which training can improve performance under experimental conditions ('what could be'), we ask about the origins of expertise as it exists in the world ('what is'). We used the twin method to investigate the genetic and environmental origins of exceptional performance in reading, a skill that is a major focus of educational training in the early school years. Selecting reading experts as the top 5% from a sample of 10,000 12-year-olds twins assessed on a battery of reading tests, three findings stand out. First, we found that genetic factors account for more than half of the difference in performance between expert and normal readers. Second, our results suggest that reading expertise is the quantitative extreme of the same genetic and environmental factors that affect reading performance for normal readers. Third, growing up in the same family and attending the same schools account for less than a fifth of the difference between expert and normal readers. We discuss implications and interpretations ('what is inherited is DNA sequence variation'; 'the abnormal is normal'). Finally, although there is no necessary relationship between 'what is' and 'what could be', the most far-reaching issues about the acquisition of expertise lie at the interface between them ('the nature of nurture: from a passive model of imposed environments to an active model of shaped experience').

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Distribution of standardized composite reading scores at age 12. N = 10,698. We operationally define as ‘expert readers’ the 506 children who scored 1.5 standard deviations above the mean.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
DF extremes analysis: Investigating the etiology of expertise by comparing the regression to the population mean for MZ and DZ co-twins of experts in the top 5% of reading performance. The MZ co-twins resemble the experts in that their mean reading score does not regress very far back to the population mean. In contrast, DZ co-twins regress halfway back to the population mean. See text for explanation and interpretation.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
A twin study of initial proficiency and acquisition of expertise on a motor-skill task with feedback given over 5 blocks of trials on each of 3 days. Open squares indicate monozygotic twins, closed squares indicate dizygotic twins.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Akaike H. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika. 1987;52:317–332.
    1. Birnbaum M.H. Human research and data collection via the internet. Annual Review of Psychology. 2004;55:803–832. - PubMed
    1. Boada R., Willcutt E., Tunick R., Chhabildas N., Olson R., DeFries J. A twin study of the etiology of high reading ability. Reading and Writing. 2002;15:683–707.
    1. Boker S., Neale M., Maes H., Wilde M., Spiegel M., Brick T. OpenMx: An open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika. 2011;76:306–317. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Boomsma D., Busjahn A., Peltonen L. Classical twin studies and beyond. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2002;3:872–882. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources