Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Jul 4:8:468.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00468. eCollection 2014.

Tracking orthographic learning in children with different profiles of reading difficulty

Affiliations

Tracking orthographic learning in children with different profiles of reading difficulty

Hua-Chen Wang et al. Front Hum Neurosci. .

Abstract

Previous studies have found that children with reading difficulties need more exposures to acquire the representations needed to support fluent reading than typically developing readers (e.g., Ehri and Saltmarsh, 1995). Building on existing orthographic learning paradigms, we report on an investigation of orthographic learning in poor readers using a new learning task tracking both the accuracy (untimed exposure duration) and fluency (200 ms exposure duration) of learning novel words over trials. In study 1, we used the paradigm to examine orthographic learning in children with specific poor reader profiles (nine with a surface profile, nine a phonological profile) and nine age-matched controls. Both profiles showed improvement over the learning cycles, but the children with surface profile showed impaired orthographic learning in spelling and orthographic choice tasks. Study 2 explored predictors of orthographic learning in a group of 91 poor readers using the same outcome measures as in Study 1. Consistent with earlier findings in typically developing readers, phonological decoding skill predicted orthographic learning. Moreover, orthographic knowledge significantly predicted orthographic learning over and beyond phonological decoding. The two studies provide insights into how poor readers learn novel words, and how their learning process may be compromised by less proficient orthographic and/or phonological skills.

Keywords: developmental dyslexia; orthographic knowledge; orthographic learning; phonological decoding; subtypes.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
The dual route model and its six basic components.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Procedure of the orthographic learning task.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adams C., Cooke R., Crutchley A., Hesketh A., Reeves D. (2001). Assessment of Comprehension and Expression 6-11 (ACE6-11). Windsor: Nelson Publishing Company Ltd
    1. Baayen R. H, Piepenbrock R., van Rijn H. (1993). The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania
    1. Bailey C., Manis F., Pedersen W., Seidenberg M. (2004). Variation among developmental dyslexics: evidence from a printed-word-learning task. J. Exp. Psychol. 87, 125–154 10.1016/j.jecp.2003.10.004 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bowey J. A., Rutherford J. (2007). Imbalanced word-reading profiles in eighth-graders. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 96, 169–196 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.11.001 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brady S., Shankweiler D. P. (eds.). (1991). Phonological Process in Literacy. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc