Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Sep 9;9(9):e106317.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106317. eCollection 2014.

Identification and characteristics of signature whistles in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) from Namibia

Affiliations

Identification and characteristics of signature whistles in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) from Namibia

Hannah Joy Kriesell et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

A signature whistle type is a learned, individually distinctive whistle type in a dolphin's acoustic repertoire that broadcasts the identity of the whistle owner. The acquisition and use of signature whistles indicates complex cognitive functioning that requires wider investigation in wild dolphin populations. Here we identify signature whistle types from a population of approximately 100 wild common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) inhabiting Walvis Bay, and describe signature whistle occurrence, acoustic parameters and temporal production. A catalogue of 43 repeatedly emitted whistle types (REWTs) was generated by analysing 79 hrs of acoustic recordings. From this, 28 signature whistle types were identified using a method based on the temporal patterns in whistle sequences. A visual classification task conducted by 5 naïve judges showed high levels of agreement in classification of whistles (Fleiss-Kappa statistic, κ = 0.848, Z = 55.3, P<0.001) and supported our categorisation. Signature whistle structure remained stable over time and location, with most types (82%) recorded in 2 or more years, and 4 identified at Walvis Bay and a second field site approximately 450 km away. Whistle acoustic parameters were consistent with those of signature whistles documented in Sarasota Bay (Florida, USA). We provide evidence of possible two-voice signature whistle production by a common bottlenose dolphin. Although signature whistle types have potential use as a marker for studying individual habitat use, we only identified approximately 28% of those from the Walvis Bay population, despite considerable recording effort. We found that signature whistle type diversity was higher in larger dolphin groups and groups with calves present. This is the first study describing signature whistles in a wild free-ranging T. truncatus population inhabiting African waters and it provides a baseline on which more in depth behavioural studies can be based.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Example spectrograms of all signature whistle types identified (n = 28) from common bottlenose dolphins in Namibia.
Frequency (kHz) is on the y-axis and ranges from 0 to 48 kHz. Time (s) is on the x-axis. The scaling is the same for all spectrograms. Spectrogram settings: FFT 512, Hanning window, overlap 50%. The numbers in the top right corner of each spectrogram are the unique SW identification numbers of each signature whistle type.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Spectrograms of signature whistle SW 12 showing possible two-voice whistle production by a common bottlenose dolphin.
SW 12 was recorded in 3 different years during 7 encounters/days (2011: A–D, 2012: E–F, 2013: G). Frequency (kHz) is on the y-axis and ranges from 0 to 24 kHz. Time (s) is on the x-axis. The scaling is the same for all spectrograms. Spectrogram settings: FFT 512, Hanning window, overlap 50%. Note reverberation of whistle in spectrogram A. Loops are highlighted by boxes in spectrogram G: HF =  High frequency contour, DS =  Down-sweep contour.
Figure 3
Figure 3. GAM response curve showing the effect of group size on the diversity of signature whistle types of common bottlenose dolphins from Namibia.
Generalized additive model response curve showing smoothed fit of the relationship between signature whistle type diversity and group size per encounter, controlled for calf presence. The plot controls for the relationship of other variables, and is a result of back-fitting the algorithm used by the R-function GAM to calculate the additive contribution of each variable using nonparametric smoothing methods. Note that the y-axis is on the scale of the link function, not the measured variable. Points represent the residuals, the solid line represents the function estimated by the GAM and the area in grey shows the 95% confidence interval.

References

    1. Cortopassi KA, Bradbury JW (2006) Contact call diversity in wild orange-fronted parakeet pairs, Aratinga canicularis . Animal Behaviour 71: 1141–1154.
    1. Wanker R, Apcin J, Jennerjahn B, Waibel B (1998) Discrimination of different social companions in spectacled parrotlets (Forpus conspicillatus): evidence for individual vocal recognition. Behavioral Ecology And Sociobiology 43: 197–202.
    1. Poole JH, Payne K, Langbauer WRJ, Moss CJ (1988) The social contexts of some very low frequency calls of African elephants. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 22 385–392.
    1. Illmann G, Schrader L, Spinka M, Sustr P (2002) Acoustical mother-offspring recognition in pigs (Sus scrofa domestica). Behaviour 139: 487–505.
    1. Janik VM, Sayigh LS (2013) Communication in bottlenose dolphins: 50 years of signature whistle research. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 199: 479–489. - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources