Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion
- PMID: 25343463
- PMCID: PMC4208767
- DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110729
Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion
Abstract
Contextual information can have a huge impact on our sensory experience. The tilt illusion is a classic example of contextual influence exerted by an oriented surround on a target's perceived orientation. Traditionally, the tilt illusion has been described as the outcome of inhibition between cortical neurons with adjacent receptive fields and a similar preference for orientation. An alternative explanation is that tilted contexts could produce a re-calibration of the subjective frame of reference. Although the distinction is subtle, only the latter model makes clear predictions for unoriented stimuli. In the present study, we tested one such prediction by asking four naive subjects to estimate three positions (4, 6, and 8 o'clock) on an imaginary clock face within a tilted surround. To indicate their estimates, they used either an unoriented dot or a line segment, with one endpoint at fixation in the middle of the surround. The surround's tilt was randomly chosen from a set of orientations (± 75°, ± 65°, ± 55°, ± 45°, ± 35°, ± 25°, ± 15°, ± 5° with respect to vertical) across trials. Our results showed systematic biases consistent with the tilt illusion in both conditions. Biases were largest when observers attempted to estimate the 4 and 8 o'clock positions, but there was no significant difference between data gathered with the dot and data gathered with the line segment. A control experiment confirmed that biases were better accounted for by a local coordinate shift than to torsional eye movements induced by the tilted context. This finding supports the idea that tilted contexts distort perceived positions as well as perceived orientations and cannot be readily explained by lateral interactions between orientation selective cells in V1.
Conflict of interest statement
Figures









Similar articles
-
Awareness is the key to attraction: dissociating the tilt illusions via conscious perception.J Vis. 2014 Oct 13;14(12):15. doi: 10.1167/14.12.15. J Vis. 2014. PMID: 25311303
-
A bias-free measure of the tilt illusion.J Vis. 2015;15(15):8. doi: 10.1167/15.15.8. J Vis. 2015. PMID: 26575194
-
A tilted frame deceives the eye and the hand.J Vis. 2008 Dec 30;8(16):18.1-16. doi: 10.1167/8.16.18. J Vis. 2008. PMID: 19146284
-
The tilt illusion: phenomenology and functional implications.Vision Res. 2014 Nov;104:3-11. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.06.009. Epub 2014 Jul 1. Vision Res. 2014. PMID: 24995379 Review.
-
Space and time in visual context.Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007 Jul;8(7):522-35. doi: 10.1038/nrn2155. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007. PMID: 17585305 Review.
Cited by
-
Perception of Upright: Multisensory Convergence and the Role of Temporo-Parietal Cortex.Front Neurol. 2017 Oct 25;8:552. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00552. eCollection 2017. Front Neurol. 2017. PMID: 29118736 Free PMC article. Review.
-
Tilted frames of reference have similar effects on the perception of gravitational vertical and the planning of vertical saccadic eye movements.Exp Brain Res. 2015 Jul;233(7):2115-25. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4282-0. Epub 2015 Apr 29. Exp Brain Res. 2015. PMID: 25921228 Free PMC article.
References
-
- Barlow HB (1961) Sensory Communication. Massachusetts, MIT press.
-
- Schwartz O, Hsu A, Dayan P (2007) Space and time in visual context. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 522–535. - PubMed
-
- Ebbinghaus HZ (1897) Über eine Methode zur Prüfung geistiger Fähigkeiten und ihre Andwendung bei Schulkindern. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane 13: 401–59.
-
- Mach E (1865) Über die Wirkung der räumlichen Verteilung des Lichtreizes auf die Netzhaut. Sitzungsberichte der mathematisch-naturwissenschaftlichen Classe der kaiserlichen Akademie der Wissen-schaften 52: 303–322.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources