Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Nov 12:5:1281.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01281. eCollection 2014.

Quantifying long-range correlations and 1/f patterns in a minimal experiment of social interaction

Affiliations

Quantifying long-range correlations and 1/f patterns in a minimal experiment of social interaction

Manuel G Bedia et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

In recent years, researchers in social cognition have found the "perceptual crossing paradigm" to be both a theoretical and practical advance toward meeting particular challenges. This paradigm has been used to analyze the type of interactive processes that emerge in minimal interactions and it has allowed progress toward understanding of the principles of social cognition processes. In this paper, we analyze whether some critical aspects of these interactions could not have been observed by previous studies. We consider alternative indicators that could complete, or even lead us to rethink, the current interpretation of the results obtained from both experimental and simulated modeling in the fields of social interactions and minimal perceptual crossing. In particular, we discuss the possibility that previous experiments have been analytically constrained to a short-term dynamic type of player response. Additionally, we propose the possibility of considering these experiments from a more suitable framework based on the use and analysis of long-range correlations and fractal dynamics. We will also reveal evidence supporting the idea that social interactions are deployed along many scales of activity. Specifically, we propose that the fractal structure of the interactions could be a more adequate framework to understand the type of social interaction patterns generated in a social engagement.

Keywords: 1/f noise; long-term correlations; multifractality; multiscale interaction; perceptual crossing; social engagement.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Cumulative probability density function of the time between collisions for different types of opponents aggregated among participants and trials. Values for the regions illustrated are: (dotted line) human vs. oscillatory agent, (dashed line) human vs. shadow agent, (solid line) both participants are human players.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Probability density function of the time between stimulations for different types of opponents aggregated among participants and trials. Values for the regions illustrated are: (A) human vs. oscillatory agent (“vs. oscillator”), (B) human vs. shadow agent (“vs. shadow”), (C) both participants are human players (“vs. human”).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Example of statistical comparison between two multiscale systems. In case 2.a the behavior is statistically different from 1 at scale s2 because the intrinsic levels of activity at this scale have been increased. However, in case 2.b the statistical differences respect to 1 at scale x2 is not due to any intrinsic change in x2 but instead to a change in the relation between x2 and x1, that now presents a phase modulation from slower to fastest frequencies.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Fractal analysis calculated on interactive patterns between two participants. Values for the regions illustrated are: (A) human vs. oscillatory agent (“vs. oscillator”), (B) human vs. shadow agent (“vs. shadow”), (C) both participants are human players (“vs. human”). The examples are representative cases of the three kinds of populations in the experiment.
Figure 5
Figure 5
(A) Boxplots distribution of β and, (B) width of the multifractal spectrum Δh in the time series of the relative velocity between participants. Values illustrated refer to interactions between: a human and a oscillatory agent (“vs. oscillator”), a human and a shadow agent (“vs. shadow”) and two human participants (“vs. human”).
Figure 6
Figure 6
Boxplots distribution of β (left side) and width of the multifractal spectrum (right side) in the velocity of the players. The upper figures (A,B) represent the fractal and multifractal analysis when we take the velocity of the player. The bottom figures (C,D) represent the case when we analyze the velocity of the opponent. Values illustrated refer to interactions between: a human and a oscillatory agent (“vs. oscillator”), a human and a shadow agent (“vs. shadow”) and two human participants (“vs. human”).

References

    1. Auvray M., Lenay C., Stewart J. (2009). Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment. New Ideas Psychol. 27, 32–47 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.12.002 - DOI
    1. Bak P., Tang C., Wiesenfeld K. (1987). Self-organized criticality: an explanation of the 1/f noise. Phys. Rev. Lett. 59, 381–384. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.59.381 - DOI - PubMed
    1. De Jaegher H. (2009). Social understanding through direct perception? yes, by interacting. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 535–542. 10.1016/j.concog.2008.10.007 - DOI - PubMed
    1. De Jaegher H., Di Paolo E., Gallagher S. (2010). Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 441–447. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Di Paolo E. A., Rohde M., Iizuka H. (2008). Sensitivity to social contingency or stability of interaction? modelling the dynamics of perceptual crossing. New Ideas Psychol. 26, 278–294 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.07.006 - DOI

LinkOut - more resources