Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 May;104(5):507-513.
doi: 10.1257/aer.104.5.507.

Rational Attention and Adaptive Coding: A Puzzle and a Solution

Affiliations

Rational Attention and Adaptive Coding: A Puzzle and a Solution

Camillo Padoa-Schioppa et al. Am Econ Rev. 2014 May.
No abstract available

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Challenge Posed by Range Adaptation Notes: Panel A: Range adaptation in orbitofrontal cortex. Each line represents the average neuronal response (baseline-subtracted) plotted against the offer value. Different lines indicate different value ranges. Panel B: In this simplified model, choices result from the activity of two neurons encoding offer value A (left) and offer value B (right). When the range of juice B increases (lighter line), the offer value B cell adapts. The indifference point, for which the two cells have equal firing rate, shifts. If decisions were made by comparing firing rates, the same quantity of juice B would be chosen less frequently. Source: Panel A reproduced from Padoa-Schioppa (2009)
Figure 2
Figure 2
Dynamics of Gating Variables and Choice Process (Axes indicate the variables S) Notes: Arrows describe the direction of flow of the differential equation (2) and its correspondent for the B good, when the input I i (t) is constant over time for both goods. The two lines describe the zeros of the vector field; their intersection the steady states, thus the possible limit values of the two gating variables. Two of the steady states are stable and one (the intermediate) unstable. In panel A, the input for the two goods is the same, so with noise the two goods are chosen with equal probability. In panel B, the input of good A is higher; the set of zeroes for the S A variables shifts upward. Now a process starting at the initial condition S i = 0 for both goods is more likely to converge to the bottom-right steady state. So the probability of choosing A is higher than 1/2. With a further increase of the input for A, only one steady state will remain, and good A will be chosen for sure.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Dynamics of Hebbian Learning (Axes indicate the weights) Notes: Arrows describe the direction of flow of the ordinary differential equation (7), and the corresponding equation for good B. The environments are uniform distributions. The two lines describe the zeros of the vector fields; their intersection the steady state, thus the limit values of the two weights. In panel A, the ranges of offers of the two goods are the same, and the two steady states have equal value. In panel B the range of good A is doubled in size, that of good B is unchanged.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abbott LF, Chance Frances S. Drivers and Modulators from Push-Pull and Balanced Synaptic Input. Progress in Brain Research. 2005;149:147–55. - PubMed
    1. Cai Xinying, Padoa-Schioppa Camillo. Neuronal Encoding of Subjective Value in Dorsal and Ventral Anterior Cingulate Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32 (11):3791–808. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Dickhaut John, Rustichini Aldo, Smith Vernon. A Neuroeconomic Theory of the Decision Process. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106 (52):22145–150. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Fusi Stefano. Hebbian Spike-Driven Synaptic Plasticity for Learning Patterns of Mean Firing Rates. Biological Cybernetics. 2002;87:459–70. - PubMed
    1. Kobayashi Shunsuke, de Carvalho Ofelia Pinto, Schultz Wolfram. Adaptation of Reward Sensitivity in Orbitofrontal Neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(2):534–44. - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources