Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Dec 9:3:e04220.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.04220.

Maximally informative foraging by Caenorhabditis elegans

Affiliations

Maximally informative foraging by Caenorhabditis elegans

Adam J Calhoun et al. Elife. .

Abstract

Animals have evolved intricate search strategies to find new sources of food. Here, we analyze a complex food seeking behavior in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans (C. elegans) to derive a general theory describing different searches. We show that C. elegans, like many other animals, uses a multi-stage search for food, where they initially explore a small area intensively ('local search') before switching to explore a much larger area ('global search'). We demonstrate that these search strategies as well as the transition between them can be quantitatively explained by a maximally informative search strategy, where the searcher seeks to continuously maximize information about the target. Although performing maximally informative search is computationally demanding, we show that a drift-diffusion model can approximate it successfully with just three neurons. Our study reveals how the maximally informative search strategy can be implemented and adopted to different search conditions.

Keywords: C. elegans; decision making; drift-diffusion model; information theory; neuroscience.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Transition between local and global search in C. elegans foraging trajectories following their removal from food.
(A) Animals search the local area by producing a large number of turns before abruptly transitioning to a global search. (B) Across many animals, this transition is readily apparent in the mean turning rate. Standard error of the mean is shown as the lightly shaded region around the solid average line. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04220.003
Figure 2.
Figure 2.. Maximally informative trajectories exhibit abrupt transitions between spiral-like and straight motion towards the boundary.
(A) Initial trajectories of the model head directly towards the peak probability of finding an odor source. (B) After some period of time the model displays an abrupt transition in behavior from a spiral-like motion to a straight motion towards the boundary. (C) The log probability that the food is elsewhere consistently increases as the search progresses. The transition between local (spiral-like) and global (straight motion towards the boundary) search occurs when this probability approaches 1. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04220.004
Figure 3.
Figure 3.. Sharp Transition between local and global phases of the search.
We used a Hidden Markov model to estimate the probability that animal's behavior falls into one of two states. (A) Example analysis based on a single trajectory shows fast (several minutes) switching time between the local and global phases of the search. (B) The distribution of transition durations across a set of trajectories from different animals. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04220.005
Figure 4.
Figure 4.. Infotaxis model quantitatively accounts for the worm trajectories.
(A) The distribution of worm displacements from an initial position at the end of the local search is non-Gaussian and can be fitted using the three parameters (See ‘Materials and methods’) of the infotaxis model. (B) The same set of parameters also accounts for the cumulative distribution for the local search duration across different individual worms. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04220.006
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. Comparison of measured local search duration times with predictions based on chemotaxis and infotaxis models.
The chemotaxis model was fit by measuring the maximum and steady-state turning rates of the animal and constraining the mean turning rate. The data (blue dashed line) and infotaxis (red solid line) are reproduced from Figure 4. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04220.007
Figure 5.
Figure 5.. Foraging trajectories deviate from predictions of the chemotaxis model.
The chemotaxis model explains the reduction in the average number of turns as adaptation to low odorant concentration. (A) For different adaptation times, the predicted dynamics of turn rate can match either the slow decay in the beginning of the search or the small rate of turning at the end of the search, but not both. Black lines are predictions using adaptation while red shows experimental measurements. (B) The average number of turns is unaffected by changes in food concentration (black), in contrast to chemotaxis predictions (grey bar) and in agreement with the infotaxis predictions (dashed line). DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04220.008
Figure 6.
Figure 6.. Drift-diffusion approximates maximally informative search across a range of conditions.
(A) The log probability that the food is located elsewhere has approximately linear dynamics, resembling a drift-diffusion decision variable. The drift rate (slope) decreases with the width of the prior distribution (B) The distribution of local search duration times can be approximated by a drift diffusion model for a range of conditions. The chemotaxis model predicts an opposite shift in the local search duration times between wide and narrow priors compared to the infotaxis predictions. In both panels (A) and (B) red and black curves correspond to wide and narrow priors, respectively. (C) The drift rate increases linearly with the width σ of the prior distribution. (D) The drift rate decreases linearly with filter width L (E) The drift rate depends primarily on the ratio L/ σ. (F) Normalizing models with different filters by their prior distribution widths reveals a common strategy. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04220.009
Figure 7.
Figure 7.. A tentative neural model for near-optimal foraging.
Maximally informative foraging can be approximated by a combination of local and global search phases. Responses of a sensory neuron initiate the start of the local search. The passage of time during the local search is encoded in the intracellular voltage of an interneuron. Finally, the duration of the local search can be modulated by the release of neuromodulators. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04220.010

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abeles M, Bergman H, Gat I, Meilijson I, Seidemann E, Tishby N, Vaadia E. Cortical activity flips among quasi-stationary states. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 1995;92:8616–8620. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.19.8616. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Agarwala EK, Chiel HJ, Thomas PJ. Pursuit of food versus pursuit of information in a Markovian perception-action loop model of foraging. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2012;304:235–272. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2012.02.016. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Barbieri C, Cocco S, Monasson R. On the trajectories and performance of infotaxis: an information-based greedy search algorithm. Europhysics Letters. 2011;94:20005. doi: 10.1209/0295-5075/94/20005. - DOI
    1. Bartumeus F, Catalan J, Fulco UL, Lyra ML, Viswanathan GM. Optimizing the encounter rate in biological interactions: Levy versus Brownian strategies. Physical Review Letters. 2002;88:097901. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.88.097901. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Benedix J. Area-restricted search by plains pocket gopher (Geormys bursarisus) in tall grass prairie habitat. Behavioral Ecology. 1993;4:318–324. doi: 10.1093/beheco/4.4.318. - DOI

Publication types