Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Feb:141:35-49.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.11.005. Epub 2014 Dec 18.

The effect of age of acquisition, socioeducational status, and proficiency on the neural processing of second language speech sounds

Affiliations

The effect of age of acquisition, socioeducational status, and proficiency on the neural processing of second language speech sounds

Pilar Archila-Suerte et al. Brain Lang. 2015 Feb.

Abstract

This study investigates the role of age of acquisition (AoA), socioeducational status (SES), and second language (L2) proficiency on the neural processing of L2 speech sounds. In a task of pre-attentive listening and passive viewing, Spanish-English bilinguals and a control group of English monolinguals listened to English syllables while watching a film of natural scenery. Eight regions of interest were selected from brain areas involved in speech perception and executive processes. The regions of interest were examined in 2 separate two-way ANOVA (AoA×SES; AoA×L2 proficiency). The results showed that AoA was the main variable affecting the neural response in L2 speech processing. Direct comparisons between AoA groups of equivalent SES and proficiency level enhanced the intensity and magnitude of the results. These results suggest that AoA, more than SES and proficiency level, determines which brain regions are recruited for the processing of second language speech sounds.

Keywords: Bilingualism; Development; Language; Neuroscience; Perception; Speech.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Bar graphs of average performance in English and Spanish language assessments across groups of English monolinguals, and Spanish-English early and late bilinguals. Error bars represents standard error.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Bar graphs of average performance in the production of English words between groups of English monolinguals, and Spanish-English early and late bilinguals. Error bars represents standard error.
Figure 3A
Figure 3A
Region of interest analyses in the bilateral superior temporal gyrus and bilateral rolandic operculum in AoA × SES ANOVA. Group comparisons in each ROI at the second level of analysis (e.g., early bilinguals vs. late bilinguals) contrast the conditions of English syllables vs. baseline. Comparisons show brain activity between groups of equivalent socioeducational background (SES). Yellow: high SES; Blue: low SES. Brain areas colored in green in panel A of the bilateral superior temporal gyrus ROI display the specific coordinates that increased in activation in the bilateral STG depending on AoA and SES. ROI p-value < 0.05, cluster size k < 20.
Figure 3B
Figure 3B
Region of interest analyses in the bilateral middle frontal gyrus and bilateral inferior parietal lobule in AoA × SES ANOVA. Group comparisons in each ROI at the second level of analysis contrast the conditions of English syllables vs. baseline. The same color conventions as Figure 3A are employed here. ROI p-value < 0.05, cluster size k < 20.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Region of interest analyses in the bilateral superior temporal gyrus, the bilateral rolandic operculum, the bilateral middle frontal gyrus, and the bilateral inferior parietal lobule in AoA × L2 proficiency ANOVA. Group comparisons in each ROI at the second level of analysis contrast the conditions of English syllables vs. baseline. Comparisons show brain activity between groups of equivalent L2 proficiency level. Red: high L2 proficiency; Cyan: low L2 proficiency. Purple represents the main effect of proficiency collapsed with the contrast of high proficiency vs. low proficiency which showed increased activity in virtually the same coordinates. ROI p-value < 0.05, cluster size k < 20.

References

    1. Abutalebi J. Neural aspects of second language representation and language control. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2008;128(3):466–478. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.03.014. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Albareda-Castellot B, Pons F, Sebastián-Gallés N. The acquisition of phonetic categories in bilingual infants: new data from an anticipatory eye movement paradigm. Developmental Science. 2011;14(2):395–401. - PubMed
    1. Aliaga-Garcia C, Mora JC. Assessing the effects of phonetic training on L2 sound perception and production. In: Watkins AMA, Rauber AS, Baptista BO, editors. Recent Research in Second Language Phonetics/Phonology: Perception and Production. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing; 2009. pp. 2–31.
    1. Archila-Suerte P, Zevin J, Bunta F, Hernandez AE. Age of Acquisition and Proficiency in a Second Language Independently Influence the Perception of Non-native Speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2011;15(1):190–201. doi: 10.1017/S1366728911000125. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Archila-Suerte P, Zevin J, Ramos A, Hernandez A. The Neural Basis of Non-Native Speech Perception in Bilingual Children. NeuroImage. 2013;67:51–63. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types