Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Mar;15(2):134-160.
doi: 10.1080/1463922X.2011.611269.

Understanding reliance on automation: effects of error type, error distribution, age and experience

Affiliations

Understanding reliance on automation: effects of error type, error distribution, age and experience

Julian Sanchez et al. Theor Issues Ergon Sci. 2014 Mar.

Abstract

An obstacle detection task supported by "imperfect" automation was used with the goal of understanding the effects of automation error types and age on automation reliance. Sixty younger and sixty older adults interacted with a multi-task simulation of an agricultural vehicle (i.e. a virtual harvesting combine). The simulator included an obstacle detection task and a fully manual tracking task. A micro-level analysis provided insight into the way reliance patterns change over time. The results indicated that there are distinct patterns of reliance that develop as a function of error type. A prevalence of automation false alarms led participants to under-rely on the automation during alarm states while over relying on it during non-alarms states. Conversely, a prevalence of automation misses led participants to over-rely on automated alarms and under-rely on the automation during non-alarm states. Older adults adjusted their behavior according to the characteristics of the automation similarly to younger adults, although it took them longer to do so. The results of this study suggest the relationship between automation reliability and reliance depends on the prevalence of specific errors and on the state of the system. Understanding the effects of automation detection criterion settings on human-automation interaction can help designers of automated systems make predictions about human behavior and system performance as a function of the characteristics of the automation.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Screenshot of simulator interface
Figure 2
Figure 2
Percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type. Each point represents the percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar during alarms in the throughout condition.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type (throughout condition). Each point represents the percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar during non-alarms in the throughout condition.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type. Each point represents the percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar during alarms in the first half condition.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type. Each point represents the percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar during non-alarms in the First Half Condition.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type. Each point represents the percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar during alarms in the Second Half Condition.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type. Each point represents the percentage of younger adults who pressed the spacebar during non-alarms in the second half condition.
Figure 8
Figure 8
Percentage of older adults who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type. Each point represents the percentage of older adults who pressed the spacebar during alarms in the throughout condition.
Figure 9
Figure 9
Percentage of older adults who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type. Each point represents the percentage of older adults who pressed the spacebar during non-alarms in the throughout condition.
Figure 10
Figure 10
Percentage of farmers who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type. Each point represents the percentage of farmers who pressed the spacebar during alarms in the throughout condition.
Figure 11
Figure 11
Percentage of farmers who pressed the spacebar (y-axis) across each 15 second time bin (x-axis) as a function of Error Type. Each point represents the percentage of farmers who pressed the spacebar during non-alarms in the throughout condition.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Bliss J. An investigation of alarm related accidents and incidents in aviation. International Journal of Aviation Psychology. 2003;13:249–268.
    1. Bliss JP, Gilson RD, Deaton JE. Human probability matching behaviour in response to alarms of varying reliability. Ergonomics. 1995;38:2300–2312. - PubMed
    1. Breznitz S. Cry wolf: The psychology of false alarms. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA; 1984.
    1. Center for Disease Control. 2003 http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm.5206a2.htm Retrieved June 6, 2011.
    1. Cotte N, Meyer J, Coughlin JF. Older and younger drivers’ reliance on collision warning systems. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society, 45th annual meeting; Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society; 2001. pp. 277–280.

LinkOut - more resources