Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015;30(1-2):120-148.
doi: 10.1080/01690965.2013.861917.

A possible functional localizer for identifying brain regions sensitive to sentence-level prosody

Affiliations

A possible functional localizer for identifying brain regions sensitive to sentence-level prosody

Evelina Fedorenko et al. Lang Cogn Neurosci. 2015.

Abstract

Investigations of how we produce and perceive prosodic patterns are not only interesting in their own right but can inform fundamental questions in language research. We here argue that functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in general - and the functional localization approach in particular (e.g., Kanwisher et al., 1997; Saxe et al., 2006; Fedorenko et al., 2010; Nieto-Castañon & Fedorenko, 2012) - has the potential to help address open research questions in prosody research and at the intersection of prosody and other domains. Critically, this approach can go beyond questions like "where in the brain does mental process x produce activation" and toward questions that probe the nature of the representations and computations that subserve different mental abilities. We describe one way to functionally define regions sensitive to sentence-level prosody in individual subjects. This or similar "localizer" contrasts can be used in future studies to test hypotheses about the precise contributions of prosody-sensitive brain regions to prosodic processing and cognition more broadly.

Keywords: fMRI prosody.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure A1
Figure A1
Twelve parcels that satisfied the functional criteria (i.e., a selective response to structure in the auditory stimuli; see Methods for details) and were present in at least 9/12 subjects. (Regions #6-9 are the regions discussed in the main body of the paper and shown in Figures 1 and 2.)
Figure A2
Figure A2
Responses of the four regions shown in Figure 2 to the individual experimental conditions.
Figure A3
Figure A3
Responses of the regions not shown in Figure 2 (i.e., regions #1-5, 10-12 in Fig. A1) to the individual experimental conditions. Region numbers correspond to the numbers of the regions in Figure A1.
Figure B1
Figure B1
Sample individual fROIs for the Left Temporal Pole region. The parcel is shown in green, and the individual activations are shown in red.
Figure C1a
Figure C1a
Pitch tracks for Item #010 (real_sent = sentence; real_words = word list; jab_sent = Jabberwocky sentence; and jab_words = pseudoword list).
Figure C1b
Figure C1b
Pitch tracks for Item #050 (real_sent = sentence; real_words = word list; jab_sent = Jabberwocky sentence; and jab_words = pseudoword list).
Figure C2a
Figure C2a
Left: Average duration values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average duration values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2b
Figure C2b
Left: Average mean pitch values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average mean pitch values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2c
Figure C2c
Left: Average maximum pitch values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average maximum pitch values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2d
Figure C2d
Left: Average minimum pitch values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average minimum pitch values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2e
Figure C2e
Left: Average initial pitch values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average initial pitch values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2f
Figure C2f
Left: Average center pitch values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average center pitch values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2g
Figure C2g
Left: Average final pitch values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average final pitch values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2h
Figure C2h
Left: Average mean intensity values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average mean intensity values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2i
Figure C2i
Left: Average maximum intensity values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average maximum intensity values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2j
Figure C2j
Left: Average minimum intensity values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average minimum intensity values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure C2k
Figure C2k
Left: Average power values for words/pseudowords – averaged across the eight positions within a stimulus – across the four conditions (sentences with real words, Jabberwocky sentences, lists of real words, lists of pseudowords). Right: Average power values for words/pseudowords at each of the eight positions for the structured (black) vs. unstructured (grey) conditions.
Figure 1
Figure 1
Top: Prosody-sensitive parcels projected onto the cortical surface. The parcels show the locations where most subjects showed activation for the relevant contrasts (i.e., an effect of structure for the auditory, but not for the visual, conditions; see Methods for details). Bottom: Parcels projected onto axial slices (color assignments are the same in both views). [These parcels are available from the first author upon request and will soon be made available at: http://web.mit.edu/evelina9/www/funcloc.html.]
Figure 2
Figure 2
Sensitivity to structure in the visual vs. auditory conditions in individually defined prosody-sensitive regions. (The responses are estimated using n-fold cross-validation, as discussed in Methods, so that data to define the fROIs and estimate the responses are independent.)
Figure 3
Figure 3
Sensitivity to structure in the visual vs. auditory conditions in brain regions sensitive to high-level linguistic processing (defined in individual subjects using the sentences > pseudoword lists contrast; Fedorenko et al., 2010). (The responses are estimated using n-fold cross-validation, so that data to define the fROIs and estimate the responses are independent.)
Figure 4
Figure 4
a: Sensitivity to structure in the visual vs. auditory conditions in individually defined prosody-sensitive regions, for the subset of conditions consisting of real words: sentences and word lists). The fROIs are defined by a conjunction of two contrasts: i) a greater response to sentences than word lists in the auditory conditions, and ii) no difference between sentences and word lists in the visual conditions. (Here, as in all the other analyses, the responses are estimated using n-fold cross-validation, as discussed in Methods, so that data to define the fROIs and estimate the responses are independent.) The Sentences > Word lists contrast is significant for the auditory conditions in all four ROIs (ps<.0005, except for RPostInfTemp whose p<.05). The Sentences > Word lists contrast is not significant for the visual conditions (except for LTempPole where it reaches significance at p<.05). b: Sensitivity to structure in the visual vs. auditory conditions in individually defined prosody-sensitive regions, for the subset of conditions consisting of pseudowords: Jabberwocky sentences and pseudoword lists). The fROIs are defined by a conjunction of two contrasts: i) a greater response to Jabberwocky sentences than pseudoword lists in the auditory conditions, and ii) no difference between Jabberwocky sentences and pseudoword lists in the visual conditions. The Jabberwocky > Pseudoword-lists contrast is significant for the auditory conditions in three of the four ROIs (ps<.05) and marginal in RPostInfTemp (p=.062). The Jabberwocky > Pseudoword-lists contrast is not significant for the visual conditions in any of the ROIs.
Figure 4
Figure 4
a: Sensitivity to structure in the visual vs. auditory conditions in individually defined prosody-sensitive regions, for the subset of conditions consisting of real words: sentences and word lists). The fROIs are defined by a conjunction of two contrasts: i) a greater response to sentences than word lists in the auditory conditions, and ii) no difference between sentences and word lists in the visual conditions. (Here, as in all the other analyses, the responses are estimated using n-fold cross-validation, as discussed in Methods, so that data to define the fROIs and estimate the responses are independent.) The Sentences > Word lists contrast is significant for the auditory conditions in all four ROIs (ps<.0005, except for RPostInfTemp whose p<.05). The Sentences > Word lists contrast is not significant for the visual conditions (except for LTempPole where it reaches significance at p<.05). b: Sensitivity to structure in the visual vs. auditory conditions in individually defined prosody-sensitive regions, for the subset of conditions consisting of pseudowords: Jabberwocky sentences and pseudoword lists). The fROIs are defined by a conjunction of two contrasts: i) a greater response to Jabberwocky sentences than pseudoword lists in the auditory conditions, and ii) no difference between Jabberwocky sentences and pseudoword lists in the visual conditions. The Jabberwocky > Pseudoword-lists contrast is significant for the auditory conditions in three of the four ROIs (ps<.05) and marginal in RPostInfTemp (p=.062). The Jabberwocky > Pseudoword-lists contrast is not significant for the visual conditions in any of the ROIs.
Figure 5
Figure 5
A comparison of the effects in the left temporal pole for individually defined functional ROIs vs. for the subject-independent anatomical ROI.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adank P. The neural bases of difficult speech comprehension and speech production: Two activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analyses. Brain and Language. 2012;122(1):42–54. - PubMed
    1. Alba-Ferrara L, Hausmann M, Mitchell R, Weis S. The neural correlates of emotional prosody comprehension: Disentangling simple from complex emotion. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(12):e28701. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Albritton D, McKoon G, Ratcliff R. Reliability of prosodic cues for resolving syntactic ambiguity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1996;22:714–735. - PubMed
    1. Amunts K, Schleicher A, Burgel U, Mohlberg H, Uylings HBM, Zilles K. Broca’s region revisited: Cytoarchitecture and inter-subject variability. Journal Comparative Neurology. 1999;412:319–341. - PubMed
    1. Bader M. Prosodic influences on reading syntactically ambiguous sentences. In: Fodor J, Ferreira F, editors. Reanalysis in sentence processing. Kluwer; Dordrecht: 1998. pp. 1–46.

LinkOut - more resources