Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Mar:142:65-75.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.12.005. Epub 2015 Feb 4.

How the brain processes different dimensions of argument structure complexity: evidence from fMRI

Affiliations

How the brain processes different dimensions of argument structure complexity: evidence from fMRI

Aya Meltzer-Asscher et al. Brain Lang. 2015 Mar.

Abstract

Verbs are central to sentence processing, as they encode argument structure (AS) information, i.e., information about the syntax and interpretation of the phrases accompanying them. The behavioral and neural correlates of AS processing have primarily been investigated in sentence-level tasks, requiring both verb processing and verb-argument integration. In the current functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, we investigated AS processing using a lexical decision task requiring only verb processing. We examined three aspects of AS complexity: number of thematic roles, number of thematic options, and mapping (non)canonicity (unaccusative vs. unergative and transitive verbs). Increased number of thematic roles elicited greater activation in the left posterior perisylvian regions claimed to support access to stored AS representations. However, the number of thematic options had no neural effects. Further, unaccusative verbs elicited longer response times and increased activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus, reflecting the processing cost of unaccusative verbs and, more generally, supporting the role of the IFG in noncanonical argument mapping.

Keywords: Alternating verbs; Argument structure; Inferior frontal gyrus; Intransitive verbs; Reaction times; Syntactic movement; Transitive verbs; Unaccusative verbs; Unergative verbs; fMRI.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Regions of differential activation for (a) transitive verbs (2 arguments) > unergative and (nonalternating) unaccusative verbs (1 argument) (b) (nonalternating) unaccusative verbs > transitive and unergative verbs

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Baayen RH, Davidson DJ, Bates DM. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language. 2008;59:390–412.
    1. Babyonyshev M, Ganger J, Pesetsky D, Wexler K. The maturation of grammatical principles: Evidence from Russian unaccusatives. Linguistic Inquiry. 2001;32:1–44.
    1. Barbieri E, Aggujaro S, Molteni F, Luzzatti C. Does argument structure complexity affect reading? A case study of an Italian agrammatic patient with deep dyslexia. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2013 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000337. - DOI
    1. Barbieri E, Luzzatti C, den Ouden DB, Thompson CK. A cross-linguistic study of the Argument Structure Complexity Hypothesis (ASCH): Implications for the interplay between semantic and syntactic processing submitted.
    1. Bastiaanse R, van Zonneveld R. Sentence production with verbs of alternating transitivity in agrammatic Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2005;18:57–66.

Publication types