Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 May 1:111:136-46.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.011. Epub 2015 Feb 14.

Genetic risk for Alzheimer's disease alters the five-year trajectory of semantic memory activation in cognitively intact elders

Affiliations

Genetic risk for Alzheimer's disease alters the five-year trajectory of semantic memory activation in cognitively intact elders

Stephen M Rao et al. Neuroimage. .

Abstract

Healthy aging is associated with cognitive declines typically accompanied by increased task-related brain activity in comparison to younger counterparts. The Scaffolding Theory of Aging and Cognition (STAC) (Park and Reuter-Lorenz, 2009; Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014) posits that compensatory brain processes are responsible for maintaining normal cognitive performance in older adults, despite accumulation of aging-related neural damage. Cross-sectional studies indicate that cognitively intact elders at genetic risk for Alzheimer's disease (AD) demonstrate patterns of increased brain activity compared to low risk elders, suggesting that compensation represents an early response to AD-associated pathology. Whether this compensatory response persists or declines with the onset of cognitive impairment can only be addressed using a longitudinal design. The current prospective, 5-year longitudinal study examined brain activation in APOE ε4 carriers (N=24) and non-carriers (N=21). All participants, ages 65-85 and cognitively intact at study entry, underwent task-activated fMRI, structural MRI, and neuropsychological assessments at baseline, 18, and 57 months. fMRI activation was measured in response to a semantic memory task requiring participants to discriminate famous from non-famous names. Results indicated that the trajectory of change in brain activation while performing this semantic memory task differed between APOE ε4 carriers and non-carriers. The APOE ε4 group exhibited greater activation than the Low Risk group at baseline, but they subsequently showed a progressive decline in activation during the follow-up periods with corresponding emergence of episodic memory loss and hippocampal atrophy. In contrast, the non-carriers demonstrated a gradual increase in activation over the 5-year period. Our results are consistent with the STAC model by demonstrating that compensation varies with the severity of underlying neural damage and can be exhausted with the onset of cognitive symptoms and increased structural brain pathology. Our fMRI results could not be attributed to changes in task performance, group differences in cerebral perfusion, or regional cortical atrophy.

Keywords: APOE ε4; Aging; Alzheimer's disease; Semantic memory; fMRI.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Longitudinal changes in RAVLT Delayed Recall (left) and ICV-corrected hippocampal volume (right) for the Low Risk and APOE ε4 groups at baseline (0 months), 18 months, and 57 months.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Voxelwise subtraction of the Famous and Non-Famous Name hemodynamic response functions for the Low Risk and APOE ε4 groups at baseline (0 months), 18 months, and 57 months.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Functional regions of interest (fROIs) generated from a disjunction mask derived from the Low Risk and APOE ε4 groups at baseline (0 months), 18 months, and 57 months (see Methods). fROI region numbers correspond to numbers in Tables 3–5. BA= Brodmann’s areas; R= right, L= left, B = bilateral; SMA = supplementary motor area.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Percent MR signal intensity derived from subtraction of Famous and Non-Famous Names for 10 fROIs demonstrating significant differences between the Low Risk and APOE ε4 groups in slope (see Table 3). Numbers in brackets correspond to fROIs described in Fig. 3.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Aaronson D, Watts B. Extensions of Grier’s computational formulas for A′ and B″ to below-chance performance. Psychol Bull. 1987;102:439–442. - PubMed
    1. Bangen KJ, Kaup AR, Mirzakhanian H, Wierenga CE, Jeste DV, Eyler LT. Compensatory brain activity during encoding among older adult with better recognition memory for face-name pairs: an integrative functional, structural, and perfusion imaging study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:402–413. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bateman RJ, Xiong C, Benzinger TLS, Fagan AM, Goate A, Fox NC, Marcus DS, Cairns NJ, Xie X, Blazey TM, Holtzman DM, Santacruz A, Buckles V, Oliver A, Moulder K, Aisen PS, Ghetti B, Klunk WE, McDade E, Martins RN, Masters CL, Mayeux R, Ringman JM, Rossor MN, Schofield PR, Sperling RA, Salloway S, Morris JC. Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med. 2012;367:795–804. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2767–2796. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bookheimer SYS, MH, Cohen MS, Saunders AM, Pericak-Vance MA, Mazziotta JC, Small GW. Patterns of brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med. 2000;343:450–456. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

Substances