Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Jun;123(6):579-89.
doi: 10.1289/ehp.1408257. Epub 2015 Feb 18.

Examination of the safety of pediatric vaccine schedules in a non-human primate model: assessments of neurodevelopment, learning, and social behavior

Affiliations

Examination of the safety of pediatric vaccine schedules in a non-human primate model: assessments of neurodevelopment, learning, and social behavior

Britni Curtis et al. Environ Health Perspect. 2015 Jun.

Erratum in

Abstract

Background: In the 1990s, the mercury-based preservative thimerosal was used in most pediatric vaccines. Although there are currently only two thimerosal-containing vaccines (TCVs) recommended for pediatric use, parental perceptions that vaccines pose safety concerns are affecting vaccination rates, particularly in light of the much expanded and more complex schedule in place today.

Objectives: The objective of this study was to examine the safety of pediatric vaccine schedules in a non-human primate model.

Methods: We administered vaccines to six groups of infant male rhesus macaques (n = 12-16/group) using a standardized thimerosal dose where appropriate. Study groups included the recommended 1990s Pediatric vaccine schedule, an accelerated 1990s Primate schedule with or without the measles-mumps-rubella (MMR) vaccine, the MMR vaccine only, and the expanded 2008 schedule. We administered saline injections to age-matched control animals (n = 16). Infant development was assessed from birth to 12 months of age by examining the acquisition of neonatal reflexes, the development of object concept permanence (OCP), computerized tests of discrimination learning, and infant social behavior. Data were analyzed using analysis of variance, multilevel modeling, and survival analyses, where appropriate.

Results: We observed no group differences in the acquisition of OCP. During discrimination learning, animals receiving TCVs had improved performance on reversal testing, although some of these same animals showed poorer performance in subsequent learning-set testing. Analysis of social and nonsocial behaviors identified few instances of negative behaviors across the entire infancy period. Although some group differences in specific behaviors were reported at 2 months of age, by 12 months all infants, irrespective of vaccination status, had developed the typical repertoire of macaque behaviors.

Conclusions: This comprehensive 5-year case-control study, which closely examined the effects of pediatric vaccines on early primate development, provided no consistent evidence of neurodevelopmental deficits or aberrant behavior in vaccinated animals.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the University of Texas at Austin or the Texas Department of State Health Services.

C.N.M. and D.M. have provided consulting services as independent contractors in regard to the data analyses; neither of them has provided services to pharmaceutical companies that manufacture vaccines or to their representatives, nor have they served as expert witnesses in thimerosal or similar lawsuits. The other authors declare they have no actual or potential competing financial interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Timing of vaccine administration for the accelerated vaccine schedule in relation to implementation of neurobehavioral assessments.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Fitted values from analytical models of social behavior for all groups from 2–12 months of age.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Fitted values from analytical models for non­social behavior for all groups from 2–12 months of age.

Comment in

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Atkinson J. Boston, MA: Springer, 277–287; 1979. The development of optokinetic nystagmus in the human infant and monkey infant: an analogue to development in kittens. In: Developmental Neurobiology of Vision (Freeman RD). NATO Advanced Study Institutes Series, Vol. 27.
    1. Barile JP, Kuperminc GP, Weintraub ES, Mink JW, Thompson WW. Thimerosal exposure in early life and neuropsychological outcomes 7–10 years later. J Pediatr Psychol. 2012;37:106–118. - PubMed
    1. Bauer DJ, Curran PJ. Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: inferential and graphical techniques. Multivariate Behav Res. 2005;40:373–400. - PubMed
    1. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Methodol. 1995;57:289–300.
    1. Berman RF, Pessah IN, Mouton PR, Mav D, Harry J. Low-level neonatal thimerosal exposure: further evaluation of altered neurotoxic potential in SJL mice. Toxicol Sci. 2008;101:294–309. - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources