Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Apr;41(2):525-41.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000039. Epub 2015 Feb 23.

The effect of stereotype threat on performance of a rhythmic motor skill

Affiliations

The effect of stereotype threat on performance of a rhythmic motor skill

Meghan E Huber et al. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2015 Apr.

Abstract

Many studies using cognitive tasks have found that stereotype threat, or concern about confirming a negative stereotype about one's group, debilitates performance. The few studies that documented similar effects on sensorimotor performance have used only relatively coarse measures to quantify performance. This study tested the effect of stereotype threat on a rhythmic ball bouncing task, where previous analyses of the task dynamics afforded more detailed quantification of the effect of threat on motor control. In this task, novices hit the ball with positive racket acceleration, indicative of unstable performance. With practice, they learn to stabilize error by changing their ball-racket impact from positive to negative acceleration. Results showed that for novices, stereotype threat potentiated hitting the ball with positive racket acceleration, leading to poorer performance of stigmatized females. However, when the threat manipulation was delivered after having acquired some skill, reflected by negative racket acceleration, the stigmatized females performed better. These findings are consistent with the mere effort account that argues that stereotype threat potentiates the most likely response on the given task. The study also demonstrates the value of identifying the control mechanisms through which stereotype threat has its effects on outcome measures.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Front and side view of the virtual experimental setup for ball bouncing. Participants were positioned in front of a screen and manipulated a real table tennis racket to rhythmically bounce a virtual ball to a target height in a 2D virtual environment.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Simulation of the ball-racket system. A: Assuming sinusoidal racket movement, the racket trajectory has a segment with positive acceleration followed by negative acceleration before its peak position. B: When the racket impacts the ball during the decelerating portion of the racket's upward motion, the ball-racket system is dynamically stable. Slightly different initial conditions all lead to the same stable ball amplitude without any changes in the racket trajectory. C: If the ball impacts the racket during the accelerating portion of the racket's upward motion, the system is unstable. Different initial conditions all lead to unstable behavior where the ball finally sticks to the racket. The only way to achieve and maintain a stable pattern is to correct for errors in the ball amplitude by a change in the racket trajectory.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Schematic diagram describing the mere effort account. If the prepotent response is correct, the mere effort account suggests that stereotype threat facilitates performance for stigmatized individuals. If the prepotent response is incorrect and participants do not know, performance is debilitated. However, if participants are able to recognize that their prepotent tendencies are incorrect and have the time to correct them, performance can be facilitated.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Time series of racket (black) and ball (gray) trajectories illustrating the dependent measures. A bounce is the event between two consecutive ball-racket impacts. Error was defined as the unsigned difference between the target height and the maximum ball amplitude at each bounce. Racket acceleration at impact was defined as the racket acceleration 25 ms before the ball-racket impact of each bounce. Ball velocity at release was defined as the velocity of ball at the instantaneous ball-racket impact of each bounce. Bounces, where the difference between the ball and racket maximum positions was below 0.25 m, referred to as stuck bounces, were excluded from the data analysis in Experiments 2 and 3.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Performance measures of all participants over practice in Experiment 1. A: The means of median of error of participants in all four experimental groups. B: Means of the median racket accelerations at impact for each trial across 25 trials. C: Means of lag-1 autocorrelation of release ball velocities at impact for each trial across 25 trials.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Mean error and racket acceleration across practice and statistical comparisons in Experiment 2. A: Participant means of median errors across 25 trials. B: Statistical comparison of error between experimental groups. Error bars represent ± 1 standard error. C: Participant means of median racket accelerations across 25 trials. D: Statistical comparison of racket acceleration between experimental groups.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Mean error and racket acceleration across practice and statistical comparisons in Experiment 3. Error bars represent ± 1 standard error of the mean. The Female ST group does not receive the stereotype threat manipulation until after trial 12, as marked with the vertical line. A: Participant means of median errors across 24 trials. B: Statistical comparison of error in Blocks 1 and 2 between experimental groups. C: Participant means of median racket accelerations across 24 trials. D: Statistical comparison of racket acceleration at impact in Blocks 1 and 2 between the two experimental groups.
Figure 8
Figure 8
Simulation of the ball-racket system with time-dependent manipulation. A: If the ball impacts the racket during the accelerating portion of the racket's upward motion, the system is unstable. B, C: When the racket impacts the ball during the decelerating portion of the racket's upward motion, the system is unstable if the racket phase is less than .05ω; otherwise, the perturbed ball-racket system is dynamically stable.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Aronson J, Fried CB, Good C. Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2002;38:113–125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2001.1491. - DOI
    1. Beilock SL, Jellison WA, Rydell RJ, McConnell AR, Carr TH. On the causal mechanisms of stereotype threat: Can skills that don't rely heavily on working memory still be threatened? Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;38:1059–1071. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167206288489. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ben-Zeev T, Fein S, Inzlicht M. Arousal and stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2005;41:174–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.007. - DOI
    1. Blascovich J, Spencer SJ, Quinn D, Steele CM. African Americans and high blood pressure: The role of stereotype threat. Psychological Science. 2001;12:225–229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00340. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bosson JK, Haymovitz EL, Pinel EC. When saying and doing diverge: The effects of stereotype threat on self-reported versus non-verbal anxiety. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2004;40:247–255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0022-1031(03)00099-4. - DOI

Publication types