Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 May;233(5):1585-95.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4233-9. Epub 2015 Mar 1.

(Dis-)Harmony in movement: effects of musical dissonance on movement timing and form

Affiliations

(Dis-)Harmony in movement: effects of musical dissonance on movement timing and form

Naeem Komeilipoor et al. Exp Brain Res. 2015 May.

Abstract

While the origins of consonance and dissonance in terms of acoustics, psychoacoustics and physiology have been debated for centuries, their plausible effects on movement synchronization have largely been ignored. The present study aimed to address this by investigating whether, and if so how, consonant/dissonant pitch intervals affect the spatiotemporal properties of regular reciprocal aiming movements. We compared movements synchronized either to consonant or to dissonant sounds and showed that they were differentially influenced by the degree of consonance of the sound presented. Interestingly, the difference was present after the sound stimulus was removed. In this case, the performance measured after consonant sound exposure was found to be more stable and accurate, with a higher percentage of information/movement coupling (tau coupling) and a higher degree of movement circularity when compared to performance measured after the exposure to dissonant sounds. We infer that the neural resonance representing consonant tones leads to finer perception/action coupling which in turn may help explain the prevailing preference for these types of tones.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
a Musical notation. b Waveform. c Frequency spectra. d Spectrograms for the four chords (two consonant and two dissonant) used in the study
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Illustration of the experimental setup where the two black rectangles represent the target zones. The duration of the inter-stimulus interval is represented as the temporal gap on the diagram
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Examples of tau coupling between the tau of the movement gap and the intrinsic tau-guide. The R 2 values displayed in the top left corner are the linear regression coefficients and k values are the coupling constants
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Absolute synchronization error means averaged across all 13 participants for both sound conditions (consonant and dissonant) in the two different stimuli presentation conditions (synchronization and continuation). Error bars denote standard errors. Significant comparisons between conditions are highlighted using an asterisk (*p < 0.05)
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Spread of error averaged across all 13 participants for both consonant and dissonant conditions in the two different stimuli presentation conditions (synchronization and continuation). Error bars denote standard errors. Significant comparisons between conditions are highlighted with an asterisk (*p < 0.05)
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Circularity index averaged across all 13 participants for both consonant and dissonant conditions in the two different stimuli presentation conditions (synchronization and continuation). Error bars denote standard errors. Significant comparisons between conditions are highlighted with an asterisk (*p < 0.05)
Fig. 7
Fig. 7
a Average circularity index for all 13 participants for both consonant and dissonant conditions during the continuation phase. b Averaged data from two participants (6 and 8) when moving with consonant and dissonant metronomes during the continuation phase. The averaged normalized velocity profile plotted against normalized displacement, and shaded regions around the velocity profiles represent error bars (SEM). For subject number 6, movements are more circular in form for consonant (red dots) than dissonant intervals (black dots), while subject number 8 showed a similar pattern of movement circularity for both intervals. The blue dots indicate the velocity profile of a perfect sinusoidal movement (color figure online)
Fig. 8
Fig. 8
R 2 values from the tau-coupling regression analysis were averaged across all 13 participants for both consonant and dissonant conditions in the two different stimuli presentation conditions (synchronization and continuation). Error bars denote standard errors. Significant comparisons between conditions are indicated with an asterisk (*p < 0.05)

References

    1. Bidelman GM, Heinz MG. Auditory-nerve responses predict pitch attributes related to musical consonance-dissonance for normal and impaired hearing. J Acoust Soc Am. 2011;130:1488–1502. doi: 10.1121/1.3605559. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bidelman GM, Krishnan A. Neural correlates of consonance, dissonance, and the hierarchy of musical pitch in the human brainstem. J Neurosci. 2009;29:13165–13171. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3900-09.2009. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bidelman GM, Krishnan A. Brainstem correlates of behavioral and compositional preferences of musical harmony. Neuroreport. 2011;22:212–216. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328344a689. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bieńkiewicz MMN, Rodger MWM, Craig CM. Timekeeping strategies operate independently from spatial and accuracy demands in beat-interception movements. Exp Brain Res. 2012;222:241–253. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3211-8. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bieńkiewicz MMN, Young W, Craig CM. Balls to the wall: how acoustic information from a ball in motion guides interceptive movement in people with Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience. 2014;275:508–518. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.050. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources