Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2015 Mar 31:6:340.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00340. eCollection 2015.

Good fences make for good neighbors but bad science: a review of what improves Bayesian reasoning and why

Affiliations
Review

Good fences make for good neighbors but bad science: a review of what improves Bayesian reasoning and why

Gary L Brase et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Bayesian reasoning, defined here as the updating of a posterior probability following new information, has historically been problematic for humans. Classic psychology experiments have tested human Bayesian reasoning through the use of word problems and have evaluated each participant's performance against the normatively correct answer provided by Bayes' theorem. The standard finding is of generally poor performance. Over the past two decades, though, progress has been made on how to improve Bayesian reasoning. Most notably, research has demonstrated that the use of frequencies in a natural sampling framework-as opposed to single-event probabilities-can improve participants' Bayesian estimates. Furthermore, pictorial aids and certain individual difference factors also can play significant roles in Bayesian reasoning success. The mechanics of how to build tasks which show these improvements is not under much debate. The explanations for why naturally sampled frequencies and pictures help Bayesian reasoning remain hotly contested, however, with many researchers falling into ingrained "camps" organized around two dominant theoretical perspectives. The present paper evaluates the merits of these theoretical perspectives, including the weight of empirical evidence, theoretical coherence, and predictive power. By these criteria, the ecological rationality approach is clearly better than the heuristics and biases view. Progress in the study of Bayesian reasoning will depend on continued research that honestly, vigorously, and consistently engages across these different theoretical accounts rather than staying "siloed" within one particular perspective. The process of science requires an understanding of competing points of view, with the ultimate goal being integration.

Keywords: Bayesian reasoning; ecological rationality; frequencies; heuristics and biases; numeracy; pictorial aids; probabilities.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
An illustration of a natural sampling framework: the total population (100) is categorized into groups (5/95) and those groups are categorized into parallel sub-groups below that.

References

    1. Ayal S., Beyth-Marom R. (2014). The effects of mental steps and compatibility on Bayesian reasoning. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 9 226–242.
    1. Barrett H. C. (2005). Enzymatic computation and cognitive modularity. Mind Lang. 20 259–287 10.1111/j.0268-1064.2005.00285.x - DOI
    1. Barrett H. C., Kurzban R. (2006). Modularity in cognition: framing the debate. Psychol. Rev. 113 628–647 10.1037/0033-295X.113.3.628 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bramwell R., West H., Salmon P. (2006). Health professionals’ and service users’ interpretation of screening test results: experimental study. Br. Med. J. 333 284–289 10.1136/bmj.38884.663102.AE - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Brase G. L. (2002). Ecological and evolutionary validity: comments on Johnson-Laird, Legrenzi, Girotto, Legrenzi, and Caverni’s (1999) mental model theory of extensional reasoning. Psychol. Rev. 109 722–728 10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.722 - DOI - PubMed