Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2016 May;19(3):452-68.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12312. Epub 2015 May 25.

Rethinking the concepts of 'local or global processors': evidence from Williams syndrome, Down syndrome, and Autism Spectrum Disorders

Affiliations
Comparative Study

Rethinking the concepts of 'local or global processors': evidence from Williams syndrome, Down syndrome, and Autism Spectrum Disorders

Dean D'Souza et al. Dev Sci. 2016 May.

Abstract

Both Williams syndrome (WS) and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) have been characterized as preferentially processing local information, whereas in Down syndrome (DS) the reported tendency is to process stimuli globally. We designed a cross-syndrome, cross-task comparison to reveal similarities and differences in local/global processing in these disorders. Our in-depth study compared local/global processing across modalities (auditory-verbal/visuo-spatial) and levels of processing (high/low) in the three syndromes. Despite claims in the literature, participants with ASD or WS failed to show a consistent local processing bias, while those with DS failed to show a reliable global processing bias. Depending on the nature of the stimuli and the task, both local and global processing biases were evident in all three neurodevelopmental disorders. These findings indicate that individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders cannot simply be characterized as local or global processors.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Examples of one stimuli type (Tf), presented at three different densities: (a), (b), and (c)
Figure 2
Figure 2. The Navon task: presentation of stimuli
Figure 3
Figure 3. Example stimuli from the Fragmented Picture Completion task
Figure 4
Figure 4. Mean response time (in seconds) for correct detection of the phoneme at initial and medial/final positions for the ASD and Control groups. Error bars represent standard error of the mean
Figure 5
Figure 5. Mean response time (in seconds) for correct detection of the phoneme at initial and medial/final positions for the DS and WS groups. Error bars represent standard error of the mean
Figure 6
Figure 6. Proportion of local matches (local / [local + global]) for each group. Error bars represent standard error of the mean

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th edn. Author; Washington, DC: 1994.
    1. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th edn. Author; Washington, DC: 2013.
    1. Aslin RN, Smith LB. Perceptual development. Annual Review of Psychology. 1988;39(1):435–473. - PubMed
    1. Atkinson AP. Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:3023–3029. - PubMed
    1. Bellugi U, Bihrle A, Jeringan T, Trauner D, Doherty S. Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1990;6(Suppl.):115–125. - PubMed

Publication types

MeSH terms