Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 May 27:6:696.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00696. eCollection 2015.

Visual working memory continues to develop through adolescence

Affiliations

Visual working memory continues to develop through adolescence

Elif Isbell et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

The capacity of visual working memory (VWM) refers to the amount of visual information that can be maintained in mind at once, readily accessible for ongoing tasks. In healthy young adults, the capacity limit of VWM corresponds to about three simple objects. While some researchers argued that VWM capacity becomes adult-like in early years of life, others claimed that the capacity of VWM continues to develop beyond middle childhood. Here we assessed whether VWM capacity reaches adult levels in adolescence. Using an adaptation of the visual change detection task, we measured VWM capacity estimates in 13-year-olds, 16-year-olds, and young adults. We tested whether the capacity estimates observed in early or later years of adolescence were comparable to the estimates obtained from adults. Our results demonstrated that the capacity of VWM continues to develop throughout adolescence, not reaching adult levels even in 16-year-olds. These findings suggest that VWM capacity displays a prolonged development, similar to the protracted trajectories observed in various other aspects of cognition.

Keywords: adolescence; cross-sectional; prolonged development; visual working memory; working memory capacity.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Example stimulus displays (not drawn to scale) for a “change trial” of set size 2.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Means and standard errors of the average VWM capacity (K) estimates computed based on set sizes 4 and 6 for the three age groups in Experiment 1. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Means and standard errors of the VWM capacity (K) estimates computed based on set sizes 4 and 6 for the three age groups across exposure conditions in Experiment 2.

References

    1. Ashkenazi S., Rosenberg-Lee M., Metcalfe A. W., Swigart A. G., Menon V. (2013). Visuo-spatial working memory is an important source of domain-general vulnerability in the development of arithmetic cognition. Neuropsychologia 51, 2305–2317. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.031 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Awh E., Barton B., Vogel E. K. (2007). Visual working memory represents a fixed number of items regardless of complexity. Psychol. Sci. 18, 622–628. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01949.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Barnea-Goraly N., Menon V., Eckert M., Tamm L., Bammer R., Karchemskiy A., et al. (2005). White matter development during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1848–1854. 10.1093/cercor/bhi062 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Blakemore S.-J. (2012). Imaging brain development: the adolescent brain. Neuroimage 61, 397–406. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.080 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brockmole J. R., Logie R. H. (2013). Age-related change in visual working memory: a study of 55,753 participants aged 8–75. Front. Psychol. 4:12. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00012 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources