Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Jun;5(2):162-172.
doi: 10.1109/TAMD.2013.2255872.

Reaching for the Unreachable: Reorganization of Reaching with Walking

Affiliations

Reaching for the Unreachable: Reorganization of Reaching with Walking

Beata J Grzyb et al. IEEE Trans Auton Ment Dev. 2013 Jun.

Abstract

Previous research suggests that reaching and walking behaviors may be linked developmentally as reaching changes at the onset of walking. Here we report new evidence on an apparent loss of the distinction between the reachable and nonreachable distances as children start walking. The experiment compared nonwalkers, walkers with help, and independent walkers in a reaching task to targets at varying distances. Reaching attempts, contact, leaning, and communication behaviors were recorded. Most of the children reached for the unreachable objects the first time it was presented. Nonwalkers, however, reached less on the subsequent trials showing clear adjustment of their reaching decisions with the failures. On the contrary, walkers consistently attempted reaches to targets at unreachable distances. We suggest that these reaching errors may result from inappropriate integration of reaching and locomotor actions, attention control and near/far visual space. We propose a reward-mediated model implemented on a NAO humanoid robot that replicates the main results from our study showing an increase in reaching attempts to nonreachable distances after the onset of walking.

Keywords: Infant reaching; near and far space integration; perceived reachability; reaching and walking.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Superimposed images from two cameras showing the experimental setup for the behavioral study. The first camera (large image) was located directly above the theatre, and recorded a birds-eye view of infants’ reaching movements and provided a clear view of the moment of contact. The second camera (small image in the right corner) was placed on the side, captured the side view of the child, and was used to determine the infants’ leaning angle.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Mean proportion of trials in which 12-month-olds reached only, reached and leaned, or neither reached nor leaned. Leaning without reaching virtually never occurred and so is not shown. (a) Nonwalkers. (b) Walkers with help. (c) Independent walkers.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Attempted boundary, that is the longest distance at infants attempted to reach on .50 or more of the trials and the Contact boundary, that is the farthest distance at which contacts are made on .50 or more of the trials for infants. (a) The Attempted boundary. (b) The Contact boundary.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
General scheme of our reward-based learning architecture for near and far space representation. Two separate three-layer neural networks are used to approximate the state-action mapping function. The state here is depth-cue information, whereas action is equivalent to a specific depth estimate that can be associated with reaching and/or walking action.
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Examples of learned Q-value functions for near space representation network. A colormap shows reward predictions (Q-values) of the output neurons (x-axis, n = 18 neurons) for all distances used during training (y-axis). Figure on the right shows how the representation of near space has changed after the onset of walking; the output neurons that previously encoded the distances near the boundary of reachable space, activate now also for distances outside of the reachable space. (a) Before walking (b) After walking.
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Percent of reaches with standard deviation (range of error bars) for infants with nonwalkers (on the left) and independent walkers (on the right) to 5 different distances (30, 37, 47, 60, and 70 cm). The computational model was tested with the use of the NAO humanoid robot, and the results are provided here for comparison. Since the NAO robot is much smaller than an average 12-month-old infant, testing distances were adjusted accordingly (13, 15, 21, 27, and 29 cm). Additionally, we tested the robot with two far distances (37 and 39 cm). (a) Nonwalkers. (b) Walkers.

References

    1. Berti A, Frassinetti F. When far becomes near: remapping of space by tool use. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12(3):415–420. - PubMed
    1. Berti A, Smania N, Allport A. Coding of far and near space in neglect patients. Neuroimage 2001 Jul;14 (1 Pt 2):S98–102. 2001;14:98–102. - PubMed
    1. Higuchi T, Imanaka K, Patla AE. Action-oriented representation of peripersonal and extrapersonal space: Insights from manual and locomotor actions. Japan Psychol Res. 2006;48(3):126–140.
    1. Lamb E, Sherrod L, Sherrod L, Lamb M. Infant Social Cognition: Theoretical and Empirical Considerations. New York, NY, USA: Taylor & Francis; 1980. [Online]. Available: http://books.google.es/books?id=vtjJx\_UBJlMC, [Online]. Available.
    1. Piaget J. The Construction of Reality in the Child. NewYork, NY, USA: Basic Books; 1954.

LinkOut - more resources