Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Jul 15;10(7):e0132322.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132322. eCollection 2015.

Adolescents' Neural Processing of Risky Decisions: Effects of Sex and Behavioral Disinhibition

Affiliations

Adolescents' Neural Processing of Risky Decisions: Effects of Sex and Behavioral Disinhibition

Thomas J Crowley et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Background: Accidental injury and homicide, relatively common among adolescents, often follow risky behaviors; those are done more by boys and by adolescents with greater behavioral disinhibition (BD).

Hypothesis: Neural processing during adolescents' risky decision-making will differ in youths with greater BD severity, and in males vs. females, both before cautious behaviors and before risky behaviors.

Methodology/principal findings: 81 adolescents (PATIENTS with substance and conduct problems, and comparison youths (Comparisons)), assessed in a 2 x 2 design (

Patients: Comparisons x Male:Female) repeatedly decided between doing a cautious behavior that earned 1 cent, or a risky one that either won 5 or lost 10 cents. Odds of winning after risky responses gradually decreased. Functional magnetic resonance imaging captured brain activity during 4-sec deliberation periods preceding responses. Most neural activation appeared in known decision-making structures. PATIENTS, who had more severe BD scores and clinical problems than Comparisons, also had extensive neural hypoactivity. Comparisons' greater activation before cautious responses included frontal pole, medial prefrontal cortex, striatum, and other regions; and before risky responses, insula, temporal, and parietal regions. Males made more risky and fewer cautious responses than females, but before cautious responses males activated numerous regions more than females. Before risky behaviors female-greater activation was more posterior, and male-greater more anterior.

Conclusions/significance: Neural processing differences during risky-cautious decision-making may underlie group differences in adolescents' substance-related and antisocial risk-taking. Patients reported harmful real-life decisions and showed extensive neural hypoactivity during risky-or-cautious decision-making. Males made more risky responses than females; apparently biased toward risky decisions, males (compared with females) utilized many more neural resources to make and maintain cautious decisions, indicating an important risk-related brain sexual dimorphism. The results suggest new possibilities for prevention and management of excessive, dangerous adolescent risk-taking.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: Authors Crowley, Sakai, Mikulich-Gilbertson, and Banich have recently held, or currently hold, grants from the United States National Institutes of Health. Crowley recently served on the National Advisory Council of the National Institute on Drug Abuse, and on a Task Force of the American Psychiatric Association for drafting the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Edition 5. Dr. Sakai received reimbursement in 2012 for completing a policy review for the WellPoint Office of Medical Policy & Technology Assessment (OMPTA), WellPoint, Inc., Thousand Oaks, CA. He also served as a board member of the ARTS (Addiction Research and Treatment Services) Foundation until June of 2015. The other authors declare no competing interests. This does not alter the authors' adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Game; Participants' Behavioral Disinhibition Scores.
(a) Colorado Balloon Game. A: Decision Trial begins, yellow illuminated 4 sec. B: Chosen press executed during 0.5-second green light. Next, red light signals outcome, 3.5 sec. If left press in B, then C (counter increases 1 cent, dull sound, no change in balloon). If right press in B, then either D (counter decreases 10 cents, frowney face, popping sound, balloon shrinks), or E (counter increases 5 cents, smiley face, coin-drop sound, balloon puffs up). F: Fixation screen 2–4 sec. G: Directed Trial begins with 4-sec yellow half-light. Illuminated side (left here) indicates side to press when light turns green. Counter: current earnings. Then, the B-F sequence from the paired Decision Trial follows exactly, but participant knows that "the computer is playing the game now", and that only the counter increase of 2 cents upon the directed press affects participant. (b) 81 Participants’ BD Scores. Crosses: means, standard deviations.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Decline in risky presses as probability of loss increased.
Four groups: patient males, comparison males, patient females, comparison females. Raw (not fitted) data shown. Comparison males' error bars: red to reduce confusing overlaps. Trials required a choice: a cautious response earned 1 cent; a risky response either won 5 or lost 10 cents. Probability of winning after risky choices declined from 0.78 to 0.22 as game progressed.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Cortical regions near scalp where groups' activations differed significantly.
These regions are potentially accessible to transcranial direct current stimulation. Row labels: Cau, before cautious responses. Ris, before risky responses. Cmp, comparison participants. F, females. M, males. Pt, patients. Three rows of possible activations not shown: (a) Cau: Pt>Cmp and (b) Ris: Pt>Cmp, were both devoid of activation; (c) Cau: F>M had activation only at site marked by Green pointer tip in Ris: M>F. Significant t-values range from 2.64 (dark red) to 7.76 (white); details in Tables 4 and 5.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Three brain regions showing significant male:female x comparison:patient group interactions (cf., Tables 4 and 5).
Dashed lines, males. Solid lines, females. Cmp, comparison participants. Pt, patients. Activation intensity is the mean activation difference (Decision Trial – Directed Trial) for all voxels in the cluster, so negative values indicate relative deactivation in Decision Trials, compared to Directed Trials. Standard errors of the means for each of these points lie in the narrow range ± 0.064–0.094.
Fig 5
Fig 5. Assessing possible confounds by changing samples or procedures.
Clusters activating significantly differently in groups appear as shadows on a 2-dimensional surface; activations preceding cautious or risky behaviors are shown separately. Abbreviations: Cmp, comparison participants. F, females. M, males. Pt, patients. Row A: entire sample, data analyzed as in Tables 4 and 5. Row B: same sample, additional adjustment for depression severity (Carroll Rating Scale). Rows C-E: analyses as in Row A, but excluding: left-handed participants (Row C: 2 patient males, 1 comparison male, 3 patient females, no comparison females); or participants receiving psychotropic medications (Row D: see Table 1); or recent regular tobacco smokers (Row E: 6 patient males, 1 comparison male, 20 patient females, 1 comparison female). All images modified identically to increase contrast between the white brain figure and its gray background.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM. Individual differences in the development of sensation seeking and impulsivity during adolescence: further evidence for a dual systems model. Dev Psychol. 2011;47: 739–746. 10.1037/a0023279 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention FastStats [Internet]. Leading causes of deaths among adolescents 15–19 years of age: accidents (unintentional injuries), homicide, suicide. [cited 2013 Jan 15] CDC National Center for Health Statistics, Adolescent Health, Mortality; [about 1 screen]. Available: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/fastats/adolescent-health.htm.
    1. Steinberg L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52: 216–224. 10.1002/dev.20445 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Somerville LH, Hare T, Casey BJ. Frontostriatal maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in adolescents. J Cogn Neuroscience. 2011;23: 2123–2134. - PMC - PubMed
    1. MacKay AP, Duran C. Adolescent Health in the United States, 2007. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 2007. Publication No. 2008–1034.

Publication types