Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Jul 3:9:82.
doi: 10.3389/fncom.2015.00082. eCollection 2015.

Quantifying effects of stochasticity in reference frame transformations on posterior distributions

Affiliations

Quantifying effects of stochasticity in reference frame transformations on posterior distributions

Hooman Alikhanian et al. Front Comput Neurosci. .

Abstract

Reference frame transformations are usually considered to be deterministic. However, translations, scaling or rotation angles could be stochastic. Indeed, variability of these entities often originates from noisy estimation processes. The impact of transformation noise on the statistics of the transformed signals is unknown and a quantification of these effects is the goal of this study. We first quantify analytically and numerically how stochastic reference frame transformations (SRFT) alter the posterior distribution of the transformed signals. We then propose an new empirical measure to quantify deviations from a given distribution when only limited data is available. We apply this empirical measure to an example in sensory-motor neuroscience to quantify how different head roll angles change the distribution of reach endpoints away from the normal distribution.

Keywords: Stochastic noise; deviation from normality; reaching; reference frame transformation; sensory-motor transformation.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Noisy rotations. (A) Scatter plot of n = 5000 original normally distributed data points with μx1 = 0, μx2 = 3, ρ = 0.5 and σx1 = σx2 = 1. (B) Same data as in (A) rotated by a fixed μθ = −π/4. (C–E) (top). Results from noisy rotations of the data in (A) by the same average amount as in (B)θ = −π/4) but with different rotational standard deviations, i.e., σθ = 0.4 (C), σθ = 1 (D), σθ = π/2 (E). Bottom parts of (C–E) show contour plots of the distribution fy(y1, y2) (Equation 10).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Effect of transformation angle noise σθ and data eccentricity from the origin ||μx|| on the multi-variate normality of the transformed data, as captured by the Kullback–Leibler distance DKL (Equation 11). μx1 = 0, μx2 = 1, ρ = 0.5 and σx1 = σx2 = 1. (A) Surface plot. (B) Corresponding contour plot.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Effect of transformation angle noise σθ and scaling factor k on the multi-variate normality of the transformed data, as captured by the Kullback–Leibler distance DKL (Equation 11). μx1 = 0, μx2 = 1, ρ = 0.5 and σx1 = σx2 = 1. (A) Surface plot. (B) Corresponding contour plot.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Effect of transformation angle noise σθ and data correlation ρ on the multi-variate normality of the transformed data, as captured by the Kullback–Leibler distance DKL (Equation 11). μx1 = 0, μx2 = 1 and σx1 = σx2 = 1. (A) Surface plot. (B) Corresponding contour plot. (C) Effect of transformation angle noise σθ on DKL for different combinations of original data variances (σx1 and σx2) and correlations ρ.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Q–Q plots of the transformed data (P1) Mahalanobis distance against the original data (P0) for different values of angular transformation noise σθ. (A) σθ = 0.1. The Q–Q plot compares an ordered sample distribution with the quantiles of the standard normal distribution. (B) σθ = 1. (C) σθ = π/2. Deviations from the unity line indicate deviations from normality.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Dependency of the empirical distance D from multi-variate normality (see Equation 14) on the angular transformation noise σθ and the number of available data points n. (A) Average (Mean) empirical distance D. (B) Standard deviation (SD) of empirical distance D. (C) Coefficient of variation (CV = SD/Mean) of empirical distance D.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Experimental validation of deviation from normality measure. When the head was rolled and thus a larger reference frame transformation was needed, data deviated more from the normal distribution as compared to when the head was straight (head roll = 0). Average measures across all 7 subjects and across all six reach targets are shown for each head roll angle (means ± s.e.m.). Asterisks indicate significant differences (ANOVA with post-hoc paired t-tests, p < 0.05). Data points at −30 and 30° head roll angle were not significantly different from one another (p > 0.1).

References

    1. Blohm G., Crawford J. D. (2007). Computations for geometrically accurate visually guided reaching in 3-d space. J. Vis. 7, 1–22. 10.1167/7.5.4 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Buneo C. A., Andersen R. A. (2006). The posterior parietal cortex: sensorimotor interface for the planning and online control of visually guided movements. Neuropsychologia 44, 2594–2606. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.011 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Burdenski T. K. (2000). Evaluating univariate, bivariate, and multivariate normality using graphical procedures. Mult. Lin. Regression Viewpoints 26, 15–28.
    1. Burns J. K., Blohm G. (2010). Multi-sensory weights depend on contextual noise in reference frame transformations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:221. 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00221 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Burns J. K., Nashed J. Y., Blohm G. (2011). Head roll influences perceived hand position. J. Vis. 11:3. 10.1167/11.9.3 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources