Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Aug 12;10(8):e0132871.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132871. eCollection 2015.

Performance of Dutch children on the Bayley III: a comparison study of US and Dutch norms

Affiliations

Performance of Dutch children on the Bayley III: a comparison study of US and Dutch norms

Leonie J P Steenis et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Background: The Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-third edition (Bayley-III) are frequently used to assess early child development worldwide. However, the original standardization only included US children, and it is still unclear whether or not these norms are adequate for use in other populations. Recently, norms for the Dutch version of the Bayley-III (The Bayley-III-NL) were made. Scores based on Dutch and US norms were compared to study the need for population-specific norms.

Methods: Scaled scores based on Dutch and US norms were compared for 1912 children between 14 days and 42 months 14 days. Next, the proportions of children scoring < 1-SD and < -2 SD based on the two norms were compared, to identify over- or under-referral for developmental delay resulting from non-population-based norms.

Results: Scaled scores based on Dutch norms fluctuated around values based on US norms on all subtests. The extent of the deviations differed across ages and subtests. Differences in means were significant across all five subtests (p < .01) with small to large effect sizes (ηp2) ranging from .03 to .26). Using the US instead of Dutch norms resulted in over-referral regarding gross motor skills, and under-referral regarding cognitive, receptive communication, expressive communication, and fine motor skills.

Conclusions: The Dutch norms differ from the US norms for all subtests and these differences are clinically relevant. Population specific norms are needed to identify children with low scores for referral and intervention, and to facilitate international comparisons of population data.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Average US and Dutch scaled scores in relation to age.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-Third edition. San Antonio, TX: Hartcourt assessments, inc; 2006.
    1. Sameroff A. A unified theory of development: A dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Dev 2010;81(1):6–22. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01378.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bayley N. The Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1993.
    1. Westera JJ, Houtzager BA, Overdiek B, Van Wassenaer AG. Applying Dutch and US versions of the BSID‐II in Dutch children born preterm leads to different outcomes. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2008;50(6):445–449-10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.02067.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Chinta S, Walker K, Halliday R, Loughran-Fowlds A, Badawi N. A comparison of the performance of healthy Australian 3-year-olds with the standardised norms of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (version-III). Arch Dis Child 2014. February 6. - PubMed

Publication types