Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Aug 31:6:1298.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01298. eCollection 2015.

A reverse order interview does not aid deception detection regarding intentions

Affiliations

A reverse order interview does not aid deception detection regarding intentions

Elise Fenn et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Promising recent research suggests that more cognitively demanding interviews improve deception detection accuracy. Would these cognitively demanding techniques work in the same way when discriminating between true and false future intentions? In Experiment 1 participants planned to complete a task, but instead were intercepted and interviewed about their intentions. Participants lied or told the truth, and were subjected to high (reverse order) or low (sequential order) cognitive load interviews. Third-party observers watched these interviews and indicated whether they thought the person was lying or telling the truth. Subjecting participants to a reverse compared to sequential interview increased the misidentification rate and the appearance of cognitive load in truth tellers. People lying about false intentions were not better identified. In Experiment 2, a second set of third-party observers rated behavioral cues. Consistent with Experiment 1, truth tellers, but not liars, exhibited more behaviors associated with lying and fewer behaviors associated with truth telling in the reverse than sequential interview. Together these results suggest that certain cognitively demanding interviews may be less useful when interviewing to detect false intentions. Explaining a true intention while under higher cognitive demand places truth tellers at risk of being misclassified. There may be such a thing as too much cognitive load induced by certain techniques.

Keywords: Cognitive Load; Future thinking; deception detection; episodic future thought; investigative interviewing.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
The proportion of accurate identifications as a function of veracity and interview condition. An accurate true identification implies that observers responded “true” when the participant was telling the truth. An accurate lie identification implies that observers responded “lie” when the participant was telling a lie.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abe N., Suzuki M., Tsukiura T., Mori E., Yamaguchi K., Itoh M., et al. (2006). Dissociable roles of prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in deception. Cereb. Cortex 16 192–199. 10.1093/cercor/bhi097 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Addis D., Schacter D. L. (2012). The hippocampus and imagining the future: where do we stand? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:173 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00173 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Blair J. P., Levine T. R., Shaw A. S. (2010). Content in context improves deception detection accuracy. Hum. Commun. Res. 36 423–442. 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01382.x - DOI
    1. Blandón-Gitlin I., Fenn E., Masip J., Yoo A. H. (2014). Cognitive-load approaches to detect deception: searching for cognitive mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18 441–444. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.05.004 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bond C. F., DePaulo B. M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judgments. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10 214–234. 10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources