Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Dec:83:22-39.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.08.007. Epub 2015 Oct 8.

Reasoning about knowledge: Children's evaluations of generality and verifiability

Affiliations

Reasoning about knowledge: Children's evaluations of generality and verifiability

Melissa A Koenig et al. Cogn Psychol. 2015 Dec.

Abstract

In a series of experiments, we examined 3- to 8-year-old children's (N=223) and adults' (N=32) use of two properties of testimony to estimate a speaker's knowledge: generality and verifiability. Participants were presented with a "Generic speaker" who made a series of 4 general claims about "pangolins" (a novel animal kind), and a "Specific speaker" who made a series of 4 specific claims about "this pangolin" as an individual. To investigate the role of verifiability, we systematically varied whether the claim referred to a perceptually-obvious feature visible in a picture (e.g., "has a pointy nose") or a non-evident feature that was not visible (e.g., "sleeps in a hollow tree"). Three main findings emerged: (1) young children showed a pronounced reliance on verifiability that decreased with age. Three-year-old children were especially prone to credit knowledge to speakers who made verifiable claims, whereas 7- to 8-year-olds and adults credited knowledge to generic speakers regardless of whether the claims were verifiable; (2) children's attributions of knowledge to generic speakers was not detectable until age 5, and only when those claims were also verifiable; (3) children often generalized speakers' knowledge outside of the pangolin domain, indicating a belief that a person's knowledge about pangolins likely extends to new facts. Findings indicate that young children may be inclined to doubt speakers who make claims they cannot verify themselves, as well as a developmentally increasing appreciation for speakers who make general claims.

Keywords: Cognitive development; Metacognition; Testimonial learning.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Selection of the Generic speaker for Explicit Judgment Trials according to age in Studies 1A and 1B.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Selection of the Generic speaker for Explicit Judgment Trials according to age in Study 2.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Relationship between preference for a speaker on 2 out of 3 Explicit Judgment Trials and selection of the Generic speaker for Generalization Trials in Study 2.

Similar articles

References

    1. Bering JM. The folk psychology of souls. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2006;29:453–462. - PubMed
    1. Bering JM, Parker BD. Children’s attributions of intentions to an invisible agent. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:253–262. - PubMed
    1. Birch SAJ, Vauthier SA, Bloom P. Three- and 4-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning. Cognition. 2008;107:1018–1034. - PubMed
    1. Brooker I, Poulin-Dubois D. Is a bird an apple? The effect of speaker labeling accuracy on infants’ word learning, imitation, and helping behaviors. Infancy. 2013;18:46–68. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Brosseau-Liard PE, Birch SAJ. Epistemic states and traits: Preschoolers appreciate the differential informativeness of situation-specific and person-specific cues to knowledge. Child Development. 2011;82:1788–1796. - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources