Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Oct 28;10(10):e0141732.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141732. eCollection 2015.

Impact of Childhood Maltreatment on the Recognition of Facial Expressions of Emotions

Affiliations

Impact of Childhood Maltreatment on the Recognition of Facial Expressions of Emotions

Martina Ardizzi et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

The development of the explicit recognition of facial expressions of emotions can be affected by childhood maltreatment experiences. A previous study demonstrated the existence of an explicit recognition bias for angry facial expressions among a population of adolescent Sierra Leonean street-boys exposed to high levels of maltreatment. In the present study, the recognition bias for angry facial expressions was investigated in a younger population of street-children and age-matched controls. Participants performed a forced-choice facial expressions recognition task. Recognition bias was measured as participants' tendency to over-attribute anger label to other negative facial expressions. Participants' heart rate was assessed and related to their behavioral performance, as index of their stress-related physiological responses. Results demonstrated the presence of a recognition bias for angry facial expressions among street-children, also pinpointing a similar, although significantly less pronounced, tendency among controls. Participants' performance was controlled for age, cognitive and educational levels and for naming skills. None of these variables influenced the recognition bias for angry facial expressions. Differently, a significant effect of heart rate on participants' tendency to use anger label was evidenced. Taken together, these results suggest that childhood exposure to maltreatment experiences amplifies children's "pre-existing bias" for anger labeling in forced-choice emotion recognition task. Moreover, they strengthen the thesis according to which the recognition bias for angry facial expressions is a manifestation of a functional adaptive mechanism that tunes victim's perceptive and attentive focus on salient environmental social stimuli.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: RR is the President of Ravera Children Rehabilitation Centre (RCRC). This does not alter the authors' adherence to all the PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Response Tendency.
Tendency rate for street-children (STch) and controls (Con). * = p < 0.005. Only between groups differences are shown. For differences within groups, see text. Error bars represent SE.
Fig 2
Fig 2. General-false-alarms.
General-false-alarms rate for street-children (STch) and controls (Con). * = p < 0.004. Only differences between groups are shown. For differences inside each group, see text. Error bars represent SE.
Fig 3
Fig 3. CPM-score predicts average General-false-alarms rate.
Plots of CMP-score versus average General-false-alarms for street-children (STch) and controls (Con). * = p < 0.05.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Fear and Sadness Emotion-false-alarms.
A) Fear Emotion-false-alarms for street-children (STch) and controls (Con). * = p<0.007. B) Sadness Emotion false alarms for street-children (STch) and controls (Con). * = p<0.006. Both panels show only differences between groups. For differences inside each group, see text. In both panels error bars represent SE.
Fig 5
Fig 5. CPM-score predicts average Fear Emotion-false-alarms.
Plot of participants’ CPM-score versus average Fear Emotion-false-alarms. * = p < 0.05.
Fig 6
Fig 6. Accuracy rate.
Accuracy rate for street-children (STch) and controls (Con). * = p < 0.004. Only differences between groups are shown. For differences inside each group, see text. Error bars represent SE.
Fig 7
Fig 7. CPM-score predicts average Accuracy rate.
Plots of CMP-score versus average Accuracy rate for street-children (STch) and controls (Con). * = p < 0.05.
Fig 8
Fig 8. HR predicts behavioral performance.
A) Plot of HR versus average Accuracy rate for street-children (STch) and controls (Con); B) Plot of HR versus Anger Tendency rate for street-children (STch) and controls (Con); C) Plot of HR versus Anger General-false-alarms rate for street-children (STch) and controls (Con). * = p < 0.01.

References

    1. Silk JS, Redcay E, Fox NA. Contributions of social and affective neuroscience to our understanding of typical and atypical development. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2014;8: 1–6. 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.02.002 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Herba C, Phillips M. Annotation: Development of facial expression recognition from childhood to adolescence: behavioural and neurological perspectives. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45: 1185–98. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00316.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Boyatzis CJ, Chazan E, Ting CZ. Preschool children’s decoding of facial emotions. J Genet Psychol. Routledge; 1993;154: 375–82. 10.1080/00221325.1993.10532190 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Odom RD, Lemond CM. Developmental differences in the perception and production of facial expressions. Child Dev. 1972;43: 359–69. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/5034725 - PubMed
    1. Philippot P, Feldman RS. Age and social competence in preschoolers’ decoding of facial expression. Br J Soc Psychol. 1990;29: 43–54. 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00885.x - DOI - PubMed

Publication types