Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Randomized Controlled Trial
. 2015 Dec;44(12):2197-210.
doi: 10.1007/s10964-015-0373-2. Epub 2015 Nov 2.

Peer Influence, Peer Status, and Prosocial Behavior: An Experimental Investigation of Peer Socialization of Adolescents' Intentions to Volunteer

Affiliations
Randomized Controlled Trial

Peer Influence, Peer Status, and Prosocial Behavior: An Experimental Investigation of Peer Socialization of Adolescents' Intentions to Volunteer

Sophia Choukas-Bradley et al. J Youth Adolesc. 2015 Dec.

Abstract

Peer influence processes have been documented extensively for a wide range of maladaptive adolescent behaviors. However, peer socialization is not inherently deleterious, and little is known about whether adolescents influence each other's prosocial behaviors, or whether some peers are more influential than others towards positive youth outcomes. This study addressed these questions using an experimental "chat room" paradigm to examine in vivo peer influence of prosocial behavior endorsement. A school-based sample of 304 early adolescents (55% female, 45% male; M(age) = 12.68) believed they were interacting electronically with same-gender grademates (i.e., "e-confederates"), whose peer status was experimentally manipulated. The participants' intent to engage in prosocial behaviors was measured pre-experiment and in subsequent "public" and "private" experimental sessions. Overall, the adolescents conformed to the e-confederates' prosocial responses in public; yet, these peer influence effects were moderated by the peer status of the e-confederates, such that youth more strongly conformed to the high-status e-confederates than to the low-status ones. There also was some evidence that these peer influence effects were maintained in the private session, indicating potential internalization of prosocial peer norms. These findings help bridge the positive youth development and peer influence literatures, with potential implications for campaigns to increase prosocial behaviors.

Keywords: Adolescence; Peer influence; Peer status; Popularity; Positive youth development; Prosocial behavior.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflicts of interest The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Boys’ estimated marginal means of prosocial scores at pre-experiment and in “public” and “private” chat room assessments, by peer status experimental condition. Error bars represent standard errors. Figure shows untransformed prosocial scores (square root transformed scores were used in analyses to test the effects shown). Prosocial scores = responses to hypothetical scenarios; response options were in the form of a 9-point Likert scale and reflected increasing likelihood to engage in prosocial behavior, from 1 = not at all likely to 9 = definitely. Pre-experiment = scores provided privately pre-experiment; “public” chat room = scores in “public” in front of peers (i.e., in the presence of e-confederates); “private” chat room = scores provided privately and “offline” (i.e., after being “logged off” from chat room) following exposure to e-confederates’ prosocial responses. High-status and low-status conditions refer to experimental conditions based on the manipulated peer status of e-confederates
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Girls’ estimated marginal means of prosocial scores at pre-experiment and in “public” and “private” chat room assessments, by peer status experimental condition. Error bars represent standard errors. Figure shows untransformed prosocial scores (square root transformed scores were used in analyses to test the effects shown). Prosocial scores = responses to hypothetical scenarios; response options were in the form of a 9-point Likert scale and reflected increasing likelihood to engage in prosocial behavior, from 1 = not at all likely to 9 = definitely. Pre-experiment = scores provided privately pre-experiment; “public” chat room = scores in “public” in front of peers (i.e., in the presence of e-confederates); “private” chat room = scores provided privately and “offline” (i.e., after being “logged off” from chat room) following exposure to e-confederates’ prosocial responses. High-status and low-status conditions refer to experimental conditions based on the manipulated peer status of e-confederates

References

    1. Allen JP, Antonishak J. Adolescent peer influences: Beyond the dark side. In: Prinstein MJ, Dodge KA, editors. Understanding peer influence in children and adolescents. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 141–160.
    1. Altermatt E, Pomerantz EM. The implications of having high-achieving versus low-achieving friends: A longitudinal analysis. Social Development. 2005;14(1):61–81.
    1. Barry C, Wentzel KR. Friend influence on prosocial behavior: The role of motivational factors and friendship characteristics. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42(1):153–163. - PubMed
    1. Brechwald WA, Prinstein MJ. Beyond homophily: A decade of advances in understanding peer influence processes. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2011;21(1):166–179. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Choukas-Bradley S, Giletta M, Widman L, Cohen GL, Prinstein MJ. Experimentally measured susceptibility to peer influence and adolescent sexual behavior trajectories: A preliminary study. Developmental Psychology. 2014;50(9):2221–2227. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types