Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Nov 11;10(11):e0141106.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141106. eCollection 2015.

Common Visual Preference for Curved Contours in Humans and Great Apes

Affiliations

Common Visual Preference for Curved Contours in Humans and Great Apes

Enric Munar et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Among the visual preferences that guide many everyday activities and decisions, from consumer choices to social judgment, preference for curved over sharp-angled contours is commonly thought to have played an adaptive role throughout human evolution, favoring the avoidance of potentially harmful objects. However, because nonhuman primates also exhibit preferences for certain visual qualities, it is conceivable that humans' preference for curved contours is grounded on perceptual and cognitive mechanisms shared with extant nonhuman primate species. Here we aimed to determine whether nonhuman great apes and humans share a visual preference for curved over sharp-angled contours using a 2-alternative forced choice experimental paradigm under comparable conditions. Our results revealed that the human group and the great ape group indeed share a common preference for curved over sharp-angled contours, but that they differ in the manner and magnitude with which this preference is expressed behaviorally. These results suggest that humans' visual preference for curved objects evolved from earlier primate species' visual preferences, and that during this process it became stronger, but also more susceptible to the influence of higher cognitive processes and preference for other visual features.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Example of contour pair.
Same semantic meaning and different contour. From Bar & Neta (2006), used with their permission.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Example of content pair.
Same kind of contour and different semantic meaning. From Bar & Neta (2006), used with their permission.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Trial sequence of Experiment 1.
A fixation cross shown for 500 ms, followed by a pair of stimuli for 80 ms, immediately replaced by a pair of grey squares, and the chosen image was shown once again, centered, and enlarged.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Proportion of curved stimulus choices by humans and great apes.
From left to right, 95% confidence interval of the proportion of curved choices by humans when stimuli pairs were presented for 80 ms (Experiment 1) and until response (Experiment 2), and by great apes when stimuli pairs were presented for 84 ms (Experiment 3) and until response (Experiment 4). The red line at value .50 indicates chance-level choice.
Fig 5
Fig 5. Consistency in the choice in humans and great apes.
From left to right, 95% confidence interval of humans’ choice consistency when stimuli pairs were presented for 80 ms (Experiment 1) and until response (Experiment 2), and by great apes when stimuli pairs were presented for 84 ms (Experiment 3) and until response (Experiment 4). The red line at value .50 indicates chance-level consistency.
Fig 6
Fig 6. Consistency in the choice of curved and sharp-angled contours only in great apes.
Consistency results from Experiments 3 and 4. From left to right, 95% confidence interval of great apes’ choice consistency when stimuli pairs were presented for 84 ms for sharp-angled and curved contours (Experiment 3), and when stimuli pairs were presented until response for sharp-angled and curved contours (Experiment 4). The red line at value .50 indicates chance-level consistency.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Palmer SE, Schloss KB, Sammartino J (2013) Visual aesthetics and human preference. Annual Review of Psychology 64: 77–107. 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100504 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Townsend C, Kahn BE (2014) The “Visual Preference Heuristic”: The influence of visual versus verbal depiction on assortment processing, perceived variety, and choice overload. Journal of Consumer Research 40: 993–1015.
    1. Killinger KM, Calkins CR, Umberger WJ, Feuz DM, Eskridge KM (2004) Consumer visual preference and value for beef steaks differing in marbling level and color. Journal of Animal Science 82: 3288–3293. - PubMed
    1. Leder H, Tinio PPL, Fuchs IM, Bohrn I (2010) When attractiveness demands longer looks: The effects of situation and gender. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63: 1858–1871. 10.1080/17470211003605142 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Rosen LH, Underwood MK (2010) Facial attractiveness as a moderator of the association between social and physical aggression and popularity in adolescents. Journal of School Psychology 48: 313–333. 10.1016/j.jsp.2010.03.001 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types