Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2015 Oct 27:9:590.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00590. eCollection 2015.

Episodes, events, and models

Affiliations

Episodes, events, and models

Sangeet S Khemlani et al. Front Hum Neurosci. .

Abstract

We describe a novel computational theory of how individuals segment perceptual information into representations of events. The theory is inspired by recent findings in the cognitive science and cognitive neuroscience of event segmentation. In line with recent theories, it holds that online event segmentation is automatic, and that event segmentation yields mental simulations of events. But it posits two novel principles as well: first, discrete episodic markers track perceptual and conceptual changes, and can be retrieved to construct event models. Second, the process of retrieving and reconstructing those episodic markers is constrained and prioritized. We describe a computational implementation of the theory, as well as a robotic extension of the theory that demonstrates the processes of online event segmentation and event model construction. The theory is the first unified computational account of event segmentation and temporal inference. We conclude by demonstrating now neuroimaging data can constrain and inspire the construction of process-level theories of human reasoning.

Keywords: ACT-R/E; MDS robot; episodic memory; event segmentation; mental models; temporal reasoning.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
A diagram of the unified theory of event segmentation and representation. In the event segmentation component of the system, which operates online and in parallel with other cognitive processes, changes are detected in continuous environmental input across a finite set of perceptual stimuli, marked by X, Y, and Z in the diagram. At the onset of a stimulus, which is indicated by a black circle, a new episodic marker is constructed. The offset of a stimulus likewise yields a new episodic marker. When the system is queried for information pertaining to temporal relationships, it uses the markers to build a discrete event model. The system then scans the model to make inferences.
Figure 2
Figure 2
The robotic implementation of the ACT-R/E cognitive. (A) depicts the MDS (mobile, dexterous, social) robot in use in our lab, and shows its various sensors and effectors. (B) provides the details of the ACT-R/E cognitive architecture (Trafton et al., 2013). The architecture is an embodied extension of ACT-R (Anderson, 2007), and it interfaces the robot's sensory apparatus. ACT-R/E is composed of multiple modules that mimic components of human cognition. For example, it includes modules for maintaining goals, storing declarative memories, processing visual, and auditory input, and issuing motor commands. Each module is paired with a buffer that limits the capacity that the system can process at once, and accordingly implements a processing bottleneck characteristic of human cognition. Computational implementations of cognitive processes, such as the event segmentation system we present, are developed in ACT-R/E by constructing procedural memory representations that are executed under pre-specified conditions, and which retrieve information from or else modify the contents of the system's various buffers. In the diagram, the thin lines depict the pipeline for retrieval from the contents of the buffers and the thick lines depict the pipeline for modifying the contents of the buffers.
Figure 3
Figure 3
The process by which episodes are encoded at event boundaries. (A) Shows a diagram of a trace of activity as a function of changes in goals, locations, and people. At each change, a new episodic marker is constructed (depicted as arrows). (B) Shows the representation of each episodic marker. Episodes are linked with symbolic information that describes the perceived changes at the time of encoding. Hence, episodes are used to uniquely describe a change in goal, location, person, and object (not depicted).
Figure 4
Figure 4
The process by episodic markers are retrieved to build event models. (A) Shows the episodic representation (see also Figure 3B). (B) Shows a veridical event model that can be constructed by an unprioritized mapping from episodic markers to model structures. (C) Shows a prioritized mapping, in which the construction of a goal event takes precedence to that of other sorts of events. Additional queries can be used to revise and flesh out the prioritized event model.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Allman M. J., Teki S., Griffiths T. D., Meck W. H. (2014). Properties of the internal clock: first-and second-order principles of subjective time. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 743–771. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115117 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Altmann E., Trafton J. G. (2002). Memory for goals: an activation-based model. Cogn. Sci. 26, 39–83. 10.1207/s15516709cog2601_2 - DOI
    1. Anderson J. R. (2007). How Can the Human Mind Occur in the Physical Universe? New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    1. Anderson S. J., Conway M. A. (1993). Investigating the structure of autobiographical memories. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 19, 1178–1196. 10.1037/0278-7393.19.5.1178 - DOI
    1. Aziz-Zadeh L., Wilson S. M., Rizzolatti G., Iacoboni M. (2006). Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions. Curr. Biol. 16, 1–6. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060 - DOI - PubMed