Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2016 May;46(3):315-28.
doi: 10.1007/s10519-015-9769-8. Epub 2015 Nov 21.

Translational Epidemiologic Approaches to Understanding the Consequences of Early-Life Exposures

Affiliations
Review

Translational Epidemiologic Approaches to Understanding the Consequences of Early-Life Exposures

Brian M D'Onofrio et al. Behav Genet. 2016 May.

Abstract

Prominent developmental theories posit a causal link between early-life exposures and later functioning. Yet, observed associations with early exposures may not reflect causal effects because of genetic and environmental confounding. The current manuscript describes how a systematic series of epidemiologic analyses that combine several genetically-informative designs and statistical approaches can help distinguish between competing theories. In particular, the manuscript details how combining the use of measured covariates with sibling-comparisons, cousin-comparisons, and additional designs can help elucidate the sources of covariation between early-life exposures and later outcomes, including the roles of (a) factors that are not shared in families, including a potential causal effect of the exposure; (b) carryover effects from the exposure of one child to the next; and (c) familial confounding. We also describe key assumptions and how they can be critically evaluated. Furthermore, we outline how subsequent analyses, including effect decomposition with respect to measured, plausible mediators, and quantitative genetic models can help further specify the underlying processes that account for the associations between early-life exposures and offspring outcomes.

Keywords: Causal inference; Cousin comparisons; Developmental origins of health and disease; Fetal growth; Genetically-informed designs; Pregnancy; Sibling comparisons.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Schematic Example of a Causal Diagram for the Structural Relationship between an Early-life Exposure and Later Outcome Note. In the diagram both measured (C1) and unmeasured (U1) common causes create back-door paths between the exposure and outcome. A known mediating factor in the causal pathway from exposure to outcome also shares common causes with the exposure (U2) and the outcome (U3) respectively. The exposure-mediator association is used to illustrate how the act of conditioning (marked by a box) on a measured covariate (C2) can block the back door path from unmeasured common causes (U2), so that C2 is a confounder of the exposure-mediator association, although the variable itself is not a common cause. Lastly the diagram also serves to illustrate how the presence of common causes of the mediator and outcome (U3) make the mediator a collider (an effect of both exposure and of U3). Conditioning on the mediator would, therefore, introduce a spurious association between exposure and outcome through the common causes (U3).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Framework for Guiding Analyses of Associations with Early Life Exposures Note. Black (thick) boxes represent methods. Blue (thin) boxes represent inferences. Solid black arrows represent results. Shaded gray arrow indicates further steps.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Example of a Causal Diagram for the Relationship between an Early-life Exposure and Later Outcome in a Pair of Siblings Note. For simplicity of illustration, we assume that all measured confounders of exposure (X) and outcome (Y) for sibling pair (i) have been properly controlled for (Aim 1), so that all common causes in this diagram are unmeasured. In addition to sharing some common causes of exposure and outcome (Ui), siblings may also share causes of exposure (UXi) and outcome (UYi) respectively. All common causes of exposure and outcome that can vary in siblings (non-shared) are captured in Ui1 and Ui2 respectively. The diagram assumes no contagion from the first sibling’s status to the next (i.e. no arrow from Xi1 to Xi2, or from Yi1 to Yi2), and no carryover effect from the first siblings’ exposure to subsequent sibling’s outcome (Xi1 → Yi2). When a comparison is made “within” (or conditional on) siblings, all pathways through shared factors (Ui, UXi and UYi) will be blocked. However, restriction to differentially exposed siblings (which corresponds to conditioning on one of the siblings’ exposure) will create a spurious association between one sibling’s exposure and the other sibling’s non-shared common causes (Ui1 → Xi1 ← UXi → Xi2; where Xi1 is a collider).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Academy of Medical Sciences Working Group. Identifying the environmental causes of disease: How should we decide what to believe and when to take action? London: Academy of Medical Sciences; 2007.
    1. Allison PD. Fixed effects regression models. Washington DC: Sage; 2009.
    1. Baker P, Mott FL. NLSY child handbook. Columbus, OH: Center for Human Resource Research; 1989.
    1. Barker DJP. Mothers, babies and health in later life. 2. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone; 1998.
    1. Byrne N, Regan C, Howard L. Administrative registers in psychiatric research: a systematic review of validity studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2005;112:409–414. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources