Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Jan;42(1):95-105.
doi: 10.1037/xan0000091.

Contexts control negative contrast and restrict the expression of flavor preference conditioning

Affiliations

Contexts control negative contrast and restrict the expression of flavor preference conditioning

Joseph M Austen et al. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2016 Jan.

Erratum in

Abstract

Consumption of a high concentration of sucrose can have either a detrimental, negative contrast effect or a facilitatory, preference conditioning effect on subsequent consumption of a low concentration of sucrose, depending on the cues that are present during consumption. The role of context and flavor cues in determining these effects were studied using analysis of the microstructure of licking in mice. Exposure to a high concentration followed by exposure to a low concentration resulted in a transient reduction in mean lick cluster size, which was context dependent (Experiment 1). However, there was no change in the total number of licks or overall consumption. When a flavor that had previously been paired with a high concentration was paired with a low concentration, there was an increase in the total number of licks, and overall consumption, but no change in the mean lick cluster size (Experiment 2). Pairing a high concentration with a flavor in a particular context before pairing the context and flavor compound with a low concentration resulted in abolishing the expression of the flavor preference conditioning effect on the total number of licks and consumption (Experiment 3). These results demonstrate that although context and flavor cues have dissociable effects on licking behavior, their interaction has an antagonistic effect on the behavioral expression of memory.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Test data for Experiment 1. Total number of licks (top) and mean lick cluster size (center) are shown in 2-min time bins for each condition. The amount of sucrose solution consumed in each of the two conditions during the test trial is shown in the bottom panel. Error bars indicate ± standard error of the mean.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Test data for Experiment 2. Total number of licks (top) and mean lick cluster size (center) are shown in 2-min time bins for each flavor. The amount of sucrose solution consumed in each of the two flavors during the test trial is shown in the bottom panel. Error bars indicate ± standard error of the mean.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Test data for Experiment 3. Top panel: Total number of licks for Group Congruent (left side) and Group Incongruent (right side). Licking for Flavors X and Y are shown by black and white circles, respectively. Middle panel: Mean lick cluster size for Group Congruent (left side) and Group Incongruent (right side). Mean lick cluster size in Contexts A and B are shown by black and white triangles, respectively. The amount of sucrose solution consumed during AX and BY trials for Group Congruent, and AY and BX trials for Group Incongruent, is shown in the bottom panel. Error bars indicate ± standard error of the mean. Note that for the different measures there is change in the appropriate between group comparisons. For the analyses of total licks and consumption, the responses for AX and BX, and BY and AY, were compared between groups, but for lick cluster size AX and AY, and BY and BX, were compared between groups. Therefore, for total licks and consumption the factor of congruency refers to whether the flavor was presented in a congruent context, whereas for lick cluster size congruency refers to whether the context was paired with a congruent flavor.

References

    1. Ackroff K., Dym C., Yiin Y. M., & Sclafani A. (2009). Rapid acquisition of conditioned flavor preferences in rats. Physiology & Behavior, 97, 406–413. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.014 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Albertella L., Harris J. A., & Boakes R. A. (2008). Acquired flavour preferences: Contextual control of adaptation-level effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 227–231. 10.1080/17470210701664864 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Austen J. M., & Sanderson D. J. (2015). Memory-dependent effects on palatability in mice. Manuscript in preparation. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Capaldi E., Sheffer J. D., & Pulley R. J. (1989). Contrast effects in flavour preference learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 41, 307–323. - PubMed
    1. Capaldi E. D., Owens J., & Palmer K. A. (1994). Effects of food-deprivation on learning and expression of flavor preferences conditioned by saccharin or sucrose. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 173–180. 10.3758/BF03199917 - DOI

Publication types