Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Jan 21:6:2018.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02018. eCollection 2015.

Does Gender-Fair Language Pay Off? The Social Perception of Professions from a Cross-Linguistic Perspective

Affiliations

Does Gender-Fair Language Pay Off? The Social Perception of Professions from a Cross-Linguistic Perspective

Lisa K Horvath et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

In many languages, masculine forms (e.g., German Lehrer, "teachers, masc.") have traditionally been used to refer to both women and men, although feminine forms are available, too. Feminine-masculine word pairs (e.g., German Lehrerinnen und Lehrer, "teachers, fem. and teachers, masc.") are recommended as gender-fair alternatives. A large body of empirical research documents that the use of gender-fair forms instead of masculine forms has a substantial impact on mental representations. Masculine forms activate more male representations even when used in a generic sense, whereas word pairs (e.g., German Lehrerinnen und Lehrer, "teachers, fem. and teachers, masc.") lead to a higher cognitive inclusion of women (i.e., visibility of women). Some recent studies, however, have also shown that in a professional context word pairs may be associated with lesser status. The present research is the first to investigate both effects within a single paradigm. A cross-linguistic (Italian and German) study with 391 participants shows that word pairs help to avoid a male bias in the gender-typing of professions and increase women's visibility; at the same time, they decrease the estimated salaries of typically feminine professions (but do not affect perceived social status or competence). This potential payoff has implications for language policies aiming at gender-fairness.

Keywords: gender stereotypes; gender-fair language; grammatical gender; professional groups; social perception; status.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Perceived social status of typically feminine and masculine professions. Note that answers were provided on a 7-point scale. Higher numbers indicate higher perceptions of social status.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Salary estimates for typically feminine and masculine professions. Note that answers were given on a 11-point scale. Higher numbers indicate higher estimates of salary.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alksnis C., Desmarais S., Curtis J. (2008). Workforce segregation and the gender wage gap: is “Women's” work valued as highly as “men's”? J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 36, 1416–1441. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00354.x - DOI
    1. American Psychological Association (2010). Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. Available online at: http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/principles.pdf
    1. Arbeitsmarktservice Österreich (2015). Berufslexikon. Available online at: http://www.berufslexikon.at
    1. Becker J. C., Glick P., Ilic M., Bohner G. (2011). Damned if she does, damned if she doesn't: consequences of accepting versus confronting patronizing help for the female target and male actor. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41, 761–773. 10.1002/ejsp.823 - DOI
    1. Bem S., Bem D. (1973). Does sex-biased job advertising “aid and abet” sex discrimination? J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 3, 6–18. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1973.tb01290.x - DOI

LinkOut - more resources