Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Sep:149:116-133.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.013. Epub 2016 Mar 23.

How do thoughts, emotions, and decisions align? A new way to examine theory of mind during middle childhood and beyond

Affiliations

How do thoughts, emotions, and decisions align? A new way to examine theory of mind during middle childhood and beyond

Kristin Hansen Lagattuta et al. J Exp Child Psychol. 2016 Sep.

Abstract

The current study examined 4- to 10-year-olds' and adults' (N=280) tendency to connect people's thoughts, emotions, and decisions into valence-matched mental state triads (thought valence=emotion valence=decision valence; e.g., anticipate something bad+feel worried+avoid) and valence-matched mental state dyads (thought-emotion, thought-decision, and emotion-decision). Participants heard vignettes about focal characters who re-encountered individuals who had previously harmed them twice, helped them twice, or both harmed and helped them. Baseline trials involved no past experience. Children and adults predicted the focal characters' thoughts (anticipate something good or bad), emotions (feel happy or worried), and decisions (go near or stay away). Results showed significant increases between 4 and 10years of age in the formation of valence-matched mental state triads and dyads, with thoughts and emotions most often aligned by valence. We also documented age-related improvement in awareness that uncertain situations elicit less valence-consistent mental states than more certain situations, with females expecting weaker coherence among characters' thoughts, emotions, and decisions than males. Controlling for age and sex, individuals with stronger executive function (working memory and inhibitory control) predicted more valence-aligned mental states. These findings add to the emerging literature on development and individual differences in children's reasoning about mental states and emotions during middle childhood and beyond.

Keywords: Emotion understanding; Executive function; Individual differences; Middle childhood; Sex differences; Theory of mind.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Example of a PN trial. After hearing the story, participants were asked three forced-choice questions: (1) “How do you think [Megan] feels right now? Does she feel worried or happy?” (2) “Does [Megan] think that [the short-haired girl] is going to do something good or bad?” (3) “What do you think [Megan] will do next? Do you think she will she stay away from [the short-haired girl] or go near [the short-haired girl]?”
Figure 2
Figure 2
All possible valence-matched and valence-mismatched triads and dyads.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Proportion of trials participants formed valence-matched mental state triads. NN = two negative past events, PP = two positive past events, NP = positive then a negative past event, PN = positive then a negative past event, Baseline = no past experience. Error bars represent standard errors. All age groups formed valence-matched mental state triads above chance (.25).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Proportion of trials participants made valence-matched mental state dyads: (a) Thought-emotion dyads; (b) Emotion-decision dyads; (c) Thought-decision dyads. NN = two negative past events, PP = two positive past events, NP = positive then a negative past event, PN = positive then a negative past event, Baseline = no past experience. Error bars represent standard errors. All age groups formed valence-matched dyads above chance (.50).

References

    1. Altemus M. Sex differences in depression and anxiety disorders: Potential biological determinants. Hormones and Behavior. 2006;50:534–538. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.031. - PubMed
    1. Altshuler JL, Ruble DN. Developmental changes in children’s awareness of strategies for coping with uncontrollable stress. Child Development. 1989;60:1337–1349. doi: 10.2307/1130925. - PubMed
    1. Amsterlaw J, Lagattuta KH, Meltzoff AN. Young children’s reasoning about the effects of emotional and physiological states on academic performance. Child Development. 2009;80:115–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01249.x. - PubMed
    1. Arsenio WF, Gold J, Adams E. Children’s conceptions and displays of moral emotions. In: Killen M, Smetana J, editors. Handbook of moral development. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; Mahwah, NJ: 2006. pp. 581–609.
    1. Austin G, Groppe K, Elsner B. The reciprocal relationship between executive function and theory of mind in middle childhood: A 1-year longitudinal perspective. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00655. - PMC - PubMed