Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Nov 15:142:67-78.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.044. Epub 2016 Mar 24.

The neural representation of objects formed through the spatiotemporal integration of visual transients

Affiliations

The neural representation of objects formed through the spatiotemporal integration of visual transients

Gennady Erlikhman et al. Neuroimage. .

Abstract

Oftentimes, objects are only partially and transiently visible as parts of them become occluded during observer or object motion. The visual system can integrate such object fragments across space and time into perceptual wholes or spatiotemporal objects. This integrative and dynamic process may involve both ventral and dorsal visual processing pathways, along which shape and spatial representations are thought to arise. We measured fMRI BOLD response to spatiotemporal objects and used multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) to decode shape information across 20 topographic regions of visual cortex. Object identity could be decoded throughout visual cortex, including intermediate (V3A, V3B, hV4, LO1-2,) and dorsal (TO1-2, and IPS0-1) visual areas. Shape-specific information, therefore, may not be limited to early and ventral visual areas, particularly when it is dynamic and must be integrated. Contrary to the classic view that the representation of objects is the purview of the ventral stream, intermediate and dorsal areas may play a distinct and critical role in the construction of object representations across space and time.

Keywords: Dorsal stream; MVPA; Shape perception; Spatiotemporal objects; V3A/B.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Stimulus displays exemplifying spatiotemporal boundary formation (SBF) as used in the current experiments
A. An invisible object (red dotted circle, Frame 1) expanded and contracted. Elements entering the boundary of the object (blue arrows, Frame 2) rotated or were displaced by a small amount. The resulting percept (Frame 3) was of expanding and contracting illusory contours. B. The three shapes used in the experiment. The boundaries of the third shape could not be recovered because of the rapid modulation of the contour relative to the density of the background elements. The resulting percept was of flickering elements in a ring-like configuration, but without a clearly-defined form as for squares and circles. This served as the control, no-shape condition.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Regions of interest for a single subject's left hemisphere shown on an inflated cortical surface
Early and ventral visual areas can be seen in the image on the left; intermediate and dorsal areas in the image on the right.
Figure 3
Figure 3. Retinotopy for a single sample subject on an inflated cortical surfaces
The top row depicts intermediate and dorsal regions. The bottom row depicts ventral regions. The left column corresponds to the left hemisphere (LH) and the right column to the right (RH). Note that thresholds vary across the four images to best illustrate the boundaries between ROIs.
Figure 4
Figure 4. Activation across conditions and ROIs
Data are shown for the no-shape (white), circle (light gray), and square (dark gray) conditions for early, intermediate, ventral, and dorsal visual areas. Error bars are standard errors. Normalized (z-scored) % signal change across all voxels in the specified ROI, averaged across hemispheres, element transformations (rotation and displacement), and across all subjects. Stars indicate significant difference in a contrast between shapes (circle and square) and no shape. Diamonds indicate a significant difference in a contrast between circles and squares.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Cross-validated classification accuracy averaged across subjects. Data split by ROI for early, intermediate, ventral, and dorsal visual areas. Data shown for decoding of element rotation vs. displacement (white), shape (circle or square) vs. no shape (light gray), and circle vs. square (dark gray). Chance performance for all classifiers was 50%. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Amano K, Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO. Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102:2704–2718. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Arcaro MJ, McMains SA, Singer BD, Kastner S. Retinotopic organization of human ventral visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2009;29:10638–10652. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Avidan G, Harel M, Hendler T, Ben-Bashat D, Zohary E, Malach R. Contrast sensitivity in human visual areas and its relationship to object recognition. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:3102–3116. - PubMed
    1. Bakin JS, Nakayama K, Gilbert CD. Visual responses in Monkey areas V1 and V2 to three-dimensional surface configurations. J Neurosci. 2000;20:8188–8198. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ban H, Yamamoto H, Hanakawa T, Urayama S-i, Aso T, Fukuyama H, Ejima Y. Topographic representation of an occluded object and the effects of spatiotemporal context in human early visual areas. J Neurosci. 2013;33:16992–17007. - PMC - PubMed