Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Mar 29:7:435.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00435. eCollection 2016.

Cue Utilization and Cognitive Load in Novel Task Performance

Affiliations

Cue Utilization and Cognitive Load in Novel Task Performance

Sue Brouwers et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

This study was designed to examine whether differences in cue utilization were associated with differences in performance during a novel, simulated rail control task, and whether these differences reflected a reduction in cognitive load. Two experiments were conducted, the first of which involved the completion of a 20-min rail control simulation that required participants to re-route trains that periodically required a diversion. Participants with a greater level of cue utilization recorded a consistently greater response latency, consistent with a strategy that maintained accuracy, but reduced the demands on cognitive resources. In the second experiment, participants completed the rail task, during which a concurrent, secondary task was introduced. The results revealed an interaction, whereby participants with lesser levels of cue utilization recorded an increase in response latency that exceeded the response latency recorded for participants with greater levels of cue utilization. The relative consistency of response latencies for participants with greater levels of cue utilization, across all blocks, despite the imposition of a secondary task, suggested that those participants with greater levels of cue utilization had adopted a strategy that was effectively minimizing the impact of additional sources of cognitive load on their performance.

Keywords: cognitive load; cognitive resources; cue utilization; workload.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
The simulated train-control display as viewed by participants. The four long, horizontal green lines represent railway tracks. The white portions on each track are the intersection lines, which are controlled by an interlocking switch labeled, “Change”. This switch is depicted by a small circle icon, located above each track. If a participant selects the “Change” icon, any train traveling on the track beneath it, will be diverted onto the intersecting line.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Rail task response latencies by cue utilization typology and block number for Experiment 1. Error bars represent ±1 SE.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Rail task response latencies by cue utilization typology and block number for Experiment 2. Error bars represent ±1 SE.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Mental workload across task conditions, by cue utilization typology. Error bars represent ±1 SE.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abernethy B. (1987). Anticipation in sport: a review. Phys. Educ. Rev. 10 5–16.
    1. Abernethy B. (1990). Anticipation in squash: differences in advance cue utilization between expert and novice players. J. Sports Sci. 8 17–34. 10.1080/02640419008732128 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ackerman B. P., Rathburn J. (1984). The effect of recognition experience on cued recall in children and adults. Child Dev. 55 1855–1864. 10.2307/1129932 - DOI
    1. Ackerman P. L. (1986). Individual differences in information processing: an investigation of intellectual abilities and task performance during practice. Intelligence 10 101–139. 10.1016/0160-2896(86)90010-3 - DOI
    1. Ackerman P. L. (2007). New developments in understanding skilled performance. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Res. 16 235–239. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00511.x - DOI

LinkOut - more resources