Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 May 3;12(5):e1004903.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004903. eCollection 2016 May.

Canonical Cortical Circuit Model Explains Rivalry, Intermittent Rivalry, and Rivalry Memory

Affiliations

Canonical Cortical Circuit Model Explains Rivalry, Intermittent Rivalry, and Rivalry Memory

Shashaank Vattikuti et al. PLoS Comput Biol. .

Abstract

It has been shown that the same canonical cortical circuit model with mutual inhibition and a fatigue process can explain perceptual rivalry and other neurophysiological responses to a range of static stimuli. However, it has been proposed that this model cannot explain responses to dynamic inputs such as found in intermittent rivalry and rivalry memory, where maintenance of a percept when the stimulus is absent is required. This challenges the universality of the basic canonical cortical circuit. Here, we show that by including an overlooked realistic small nonspecific background neural activity, the same basic model can reproduce intermittent rivalry and rivalry memory without compromising static rivalry and other cortical phenomena. The background activity induces a mutual-inhibition mechanism for short-term memory, which is robust to noise and where fine-tuning of recurrent excitation or inclusion of sub-threshold currents or synaptic facilitation is unnecessary. We prove existence conditions for the mechanism and show that it can explain experimental results from the quartet apparent motion illusion, which is a prototypical intermittent rivalry stimulus.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Quartet illusion.
We presented a variant of the quartet consisting of nested quartets. A single quartet frame consists of two dots located at opposing corners of a square, across the diagonal. In the next frame the other set of opposing dots is shown. A single transition between frames is perceived as either a motion with the dots moving vertically together as an illusory bar rotating clockwise (top right frame), or horizontally as a bar rotating counterclockwise (bottom right frame).
Fig 2
Fig 2. Paths of motion and neural network for the quartet illusion.
(a) Each frame transition of the quartet illusion (t0, t1, t2,…) induces an ambiguous percept of one of two possible motions characterized by an orientation—horizontal (H) or vertical (V)—and a direction of rotation—clockwise (+) or counter-clockwise (-). During a frame transition there is competition (mutual inhibition) between the two possible perceived motions—V+ competes with H- and V- competes with H+. In the illusion, once an orientation becomes dominant, there is an oscillation in direction on each frame presentation, e.g. V+, V-, V+, etc. Eventually, the other orientation becomes dominant and the oscillations in direction will continue, e.g. H+, H-, H+, etc. The rivalry refers to the alternation between orientations. (b) A table of the allowed transitions due to the symmetries in the illusion. Each row indicates the perceived motion at the current transition and each column the perceived motion at the next transition; a value of one in a column indicates a possible path. (c) The circuit can be reduced by averaging over the fast direction oscillations into a two pool circuit with mutual inhibition between competing neuronal pools representing H and V orientations with an intermittent drive (dashed lines).
Fig 3
Fig 3. Dynamic L4 and habituation.
Violin plots of percept duration (TD in movie frames) versus the interval between changes in frame (Tframe) for author-subjects A11 (a) and A10 (b) and versus percept epochs (order of reported percepts) for A10 (c). The width of the violin plot represents the probability distribution for a dominance duration. (a-b) Increased TD with Tframe (dynamic L4). (c) Decreased TD across percept epochs (habituation) for pooled Tframe data.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Dynamic L4 survival analysis for pooled hypothesis-naive subject data.
(a) Violin plots with means (diamonds) for TD versus Tframe. TD is the reported uninterrupted duration of either horizontal or vertical motion (or for a few subjects rotational motion), in units of the number of presented movie frames. Tframe is the interval between movie frames. (b) Mean of the pooled data. (c) Tframe TD-survival plots. There is a small but significant difference in survival probabilities.
Fig 5
Fig 5. Habituation survival analysis for pooled hypothesis-naive subject data.
(a) Violin plots for pooled data of TD vs. percept epoch (the order of reported percepts). (b) Survival plots for the same analysis for various epochs.
Fig 6
Fig 6. Persistent activity maintains memory.
a) Dominance durations (TD) as a function of percept epochs starting from rest for different Tframe intervals and fatigue time constants. A subset of the Tframe conditions tested showed habituation. When the fatigue time constant was increased there was both an overall increase in dominance durations and all of the rivalry states showed habituation. See S2 Text for model specifications. b) Like-orientated pools are bistable. When dominant (and receiving nominal inhibition from the suppressed pools) one set of like-orientated pools (H or V) can have persistent activity. Here the pools were initialized with zero activity (no input), then one of the two (green) was given a brief input (pulse), followed by no input, and then a hard reset of their activity to zero. The pulse elicited non-zero activity in both pools but was greater for the pool with direct input. Then instead of falling back to zero activity, both pools remained in an elevated state despite a lack of input. When their activity was forced to zero (reset), they then remained inactive.
Fig 7
Fig 7. Background-activity stabilizes memory.
a)(Top) No background activity results in fast switching between orientations for frame interval (Tframe) of 200 milliseconds. (Bottom) Inclusion of small nonspecific background input results in non-zero activity during off-states (same on-state input as in above). Orientation persists for multiple frame transitions. b) Dominance duration (TD) increases with Tframe. All cases show habituation over percept epochs with a larger effect for short intervals. See S2 Text for model specifications.
Fig 8
Fig 8. Dynamic L4 for two-pool system under different assumptions.
Rate models with either fixed or noisy background input (Soff) and local or nonlocal fatigue and a conductance-based model with local fatigue all showed dynamic L4. For Tframe to the right of a curve we observed no alternations for the duration of the simulation. See S2 Text for model specifications.
Fig 9
Fig 9. Habituation of percept duration over percept epochs.
Simulation results using local-fatigue only or nonlocal fatigue only models with the variables initialized at resting state. For local fatigue there is a decrease in the dominance duration (TD) for subsequent dominance periods. For example the first dominance duration (epoch one) is longer than the second. This was seen for both the rate model and the spiking model. For nonlocal fatigue in the rate model the first epoch is shorter than the following epochs. See S2 Text for model specifications.
Fig 10
Fig 10. u1-u2 bifurcations across static symmetric external drive S for system with nonlocal fatigue.
The pitchfork bifurcation for arbitrarily low input provides the asymmetry necessary for intermittent rivalry and rivalry memory. It allows for memory of the last dominant state by maintaining asymmetry for very small (background) input (Soff) (Soff = 0.001). A shift in the asymmetric state in the off-state by a build-up of fatigue due to larger S in the on-state (Son) eventually leads to a shift in dominance. Although not drawn, the limit cycle extends to the red pitchfork terminating in a SNIC. A smoothed threshold was used to generate the plot. For actual simulations we used a hard threshold and zero input gives symmetric, zero activity.
Fig 11
Fig 11. Local versus nonlocal fatigue variables for the rested rate-model.
(a) Local fatigue variable a initialized to zero for pools i and j over percept epochs (Tframe = 105 milliseconds). (b) Nonlocal fatigue variable s initialized to one for pools i and j over percept epochs (Tframe = 150 milliseconds). See S2 Text for other model specifications.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ling S, Blake R. Normalization regulates competition for visual awareness. Neuron. Elsevier; 2012;75: 531–40. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Leopold DA, Logothetis NK. Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys’ percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature. 1996;379: 549–553. - PubMed
    1. Leopold DA, Maier A, LN K. Measuring Subjective Visual Perception in the Nonhuman Primate. J Conscious Stud. 2003;10.
    1. Tong F, Meng M, Blake R. Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10: 502–511. - PubMed
    1. Blake R, Logothetis NK. Visual competition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3: 13–21. - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources