Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Jun 6:6:27252.
doi: 10.1038/srep27252.

Perceptual Modalities Guiding Bat Flight in a Native Habitat

Affiliations

Perceptual Modalities Guiding Bat Flight in a Native Habitat

Zhaodan Kong et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Flying animals accomplish high-speed navigation through fields of obstacles using a suite of sensory modalities that blend spatial memory with input from vision, tactile sensing, and, in the case of most bats and some other animals, echolocation. Although a good deal of previous research has been focused on the role of individual modes of sensing in animal locomotion, our understanding of sensory integration and the interplay among modalities is still meager. To understand how bats integrate sensory input from echolocation, vision, and spatial memory, we conducted an experiment in which bats flying in their natural habitat were challenged over the course of several evening emergences with a novel obstacle placed in their flight path. Our analysis of reconstructed flight data suggests that vision, echolocation, and spatial memory together with the possible exercise of an ability in using predictive navigation are mutually reinforcing aspects of a composite perceptual system that guides flight. Together with the recent development in robotics, our paper points to the possible interpretation that while each stream of sensory information plays an important role in bat navigation, it is the emergent effects of combining modalities that enable bats to fly through complex spaces.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Audio and video data are synchronized and then overlaid with the environment to study the variations of bat behavior spatially and temporally.
The flight corridor is about 7 meters long and 4 meters wide with Myotis velifer flying from the right (where their cave roost is located) to the left (towards their foraging area). A PVC pipe was placed in the middle of the flight corridor from day 2 to day 6. Three dimensional flight trajectories (colored curves are sample tracks from day 2) were then synchronized with bat calls (their locations are shown as red dots) during post-processing of data. –Photograph background by Z. Kong; three dimensional trajectories and overall image composition by S. Wang. The upper-right inset shows some reconstructed trajectories on day 2 where red triangles indicate the locations of hot-pads and red circle indicates the location of the hot-pad attached to the pole.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Histograms showing the effects of spatial memory on the flight behavior of bats.
The histograms of (a) mean call rate (in Hz), (b) mean speed (in meter per second) and (c) mean turning rate (in radian per second) are shown for days 1, 2, 4, 6, and 7. Black triangles mark key feature locations (e.g., tree branches). Black dots mark locations of the obstacle (its virtual locations on days 1 and 7 were shown for comparison). The evolution of (d) call rate and (e) speed is shown for the positions marked as black squares in (a,b). (f) shows the occupancy histograms of day 1 and day 7. The black dots mark the virtual locations of the pole.
Figure 3
Figure 3. Summary of the heading delay definition for a leader-follower bat pair.
(a) is a cartoon representation of a typical leader-follower pair with the red bat being the leader (bat i) and the yellow one being the follower (bat j). The arrows indicate the direction of motion at each time frame (with ∆ as the sampling time). For the bat pair (i, j), the heading correlation function is given by formula image. The heading delay (a concept similar to the directional correlation delay introduced by Nagy et al.19) of the bat pair is then defined as formula image that corresponds to the maximum value of Cij(τ) as visualized in (b).
Figure 4
Figure 4. Heading alignment in leader-follower bat pairs is frequently too rapid to be explained by echoes returning from the followers’ vocalizations.
Horizontal axes denote follower bat heading delay values, indicating the time required for the follower to align with the leader. The green curves and blue curves show the PDFs (probability distribution function) of heading delays, and the CDF (cumulative distribution function) of heading delays, respectively. Vertical lines mark significant temporal values: red designates the end of the first call interval; black is one call interval time plus the auditory reaction time (90 ms), and the blue line is the average reaction time of maneuvering bats in a previous study (300 ms).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Lin H. T., Ros I. G. & Biewener A. A. Through the eyes of a bird: modelling visually guided obstacle flight. J. R. Soc. Interface. 11, 20140239 (2014). - PMC - PubMed
    1. Barchi J. R., Knowles J. M. & Simmons J. A. Spatial memory and stereotypy of flight paths by big brown bats in cluttered surroundings. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 1053–1063 (2013). - PubMed
    1. Straw A. D., Lee S. & Dickinson M. H. Visual control of altitude in flying drosophila. Curr. Biol. 20, 1550–1556 (2010). - PubMed
    1. Fukushi T. & Wehner R. Navigation in wood ants Formica japonica: context dependent use of landmarks. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 3431–3439 (2004). - PubMed
    1. Geva-Sagiv M., Las L., Yovel Y. & Ulanovsky N. Spatial cognition in bats and rats: from sensory acquisition to multiscale maps and navigation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 94–108 (2015). - PubMed

Publication types