Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Jun 29;11(6):e0158459.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158459. eCollection 2016.

Roses Are Red, Socks Are Blue: Switching Dimensions Disrupts Young Children's Language Comprehension

Affiliations

Roses Are Red, Socks Are Blue: Switching Dimensions Disrupts Young Children's Language Comprehension

Ron Pomper et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Language is used to identify objects in many different ways. An apple can be identified using its name, color, and other attributes. Skilled language comprehension requires listeners to flexibly shift between different dimensions. We asked whether this shifting would be difficult for 3-year-olds, who have relatively immature executive function skills and struggle to switch between dimensions in card sorting tasks. In the current experiment, children first heard a series of sentences identifying objects using a single dimension (either names or colors). In the second half of the experiment, the labeling dimension was switched. Children were significantly less accurate in fixating the correct object following the dimensional switch. This disruption, however, was temporary; recognition accuracy recovered with increased exposure to the new labeling dimension. These findings provide the first evidence that children's difficulty in shifting between dimensions impacts their ability to comprehend speech. This limitation may affect children's ability to form rich, multi-dimensional representations when learning new words.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Mean Accuracy by Block.
Proportion of time spent looking to the target object out of the total time spent looking at both objects during the critical window for trials before the dimensional change (Pre-Switch) and after the dimensional change (Post-Switch). Chance = 0.5. Data points represent the proportion for each subject averaged across trials. Error bars represent +/- 1 SE.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Mean Accuracy by Block Halves.
Proportion of time spent looking to the target object out of the total time spent looking at both objects during the critical window for trials in the first half and second half of the Pre-Switch and Post-Switch blocks. Chance = 0.5. Data points represent the proportion for each subject averaged across trials. Error bars represent +/- 1 SE.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Time Course of Fixations by Block.
Time course of fixations to the target object on Pre-Switch and Post-Switch trials. Fixations are plotted as the empirical log-odds averaged across participants. Chance = 0 log-odds. Data points are observed behavioral data and lines the growth curve fits (ribbons around the lines indicate +/- 1 SE).
Fig 4
Fig 4. Time Course of Fixations by Block Halves.
Time course of fixations to the target object on trials in the first half and second half of the Pre-Switch and Post-Switch blocks. Fixations are plotted as the empirical log-odds averaged across participants. Chance = 0 log-odds. Data points are observed behavioral data and lines the growth curve fits (ribbons around the lines indicate +/- 1 SE).
Fig 5
Fig 5. Mean Accuracy by Block by EF.
Proportion of time spent looking to the target object out of the total time spent looking at both objects during the critical window for Pre-Switch and Post-Switch trials for children with low EF (left panel) compared to high EF (right panel). Chance = 0.5. Data points represent the proportion for each subject averaged across trials. Error bars represent +/- 1 SE.
Fig 6
Fig 6. Time Course of Fixations by Block by EF.
Time course of fixations to the target object on Pre-Switch and Post-Switch trials for children with low EF (left panel) compared to high EF (right panel). Fixations are plotted as the empirical log-odds averaged across participants. Chance = 0 log-odds. Data points are observed behavioral data and lines the growth curve fits (ribbons around the lines indicate +/- 1 SE).
Fig 7
Fig 7. Mean Accuracy by Block Halves by EF.
Proportion of time spent looking to the target object out of the total time spent looking at both objects during the critical window for trials in the first and second half of the Pre-Switch and Post-Switch blocks. Children with low EF are plotted in the left panel and high EF in the right panel. Chance = 0.5. Data points represent the proportion for each subject averaged across trials. Error bars represent +/- 1 SE.
Fig 8
Fig 8. Time Course of Fixations by Block Halves by EF.
Time course of fixations to the target object on trials in the first and second half of the Pre-Switch and Post-Switch blocks. Children with low EF are plotted in the left panel and children with high EF in the right panel. Fixations are plotted as the empirical log-odds averaged across participants. Chance = 0 log-odds. Data points are observed behavioral data and lines the growth curve fits (ribbons around the lines indicate +/- 1 SE).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Diamond A. Normal Development of Prefrontal Cortex from Birth to Young Adulthood: Cognitive Functions, Anatomy, and Biochemistry In: Stuss D, Knight R, editors. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2002. p. 466–503.
    1. Diamond A. The Early Development of Executive Functions In: Bialystok E, Craik FIM, editors. Lifespan Cognition: Mechanisms of Change. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006. p. 70–95.
    1. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: a Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000. August;41:49–100. - PubMed
    1. Fedorenko E. The role of domain-general cognitive control in language comprehension. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1–17. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Novick JM, Trueswell JC, Thompson-Schill SL. Broca’s area and language processing: Evidence for the cognitive control connection. Lang Linguist Compass. 2010;4(10):906–24.

LinkOut - more resources