Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Sep;37(7):548-56.
doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000318.

The Effect of Bilingual Exposure on Executive Function Skills in Preterm and Full-Term Preschoolers

Affiliations

The Effect of Bilingual Exposure on Executive Function Skills in Preterm and Full-Term Preschoolers

Irene M Loe et al. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2016 Sep.

Abstract

Objective: To evaluate the effects of bilingual exposure on executive function (EF) skills, measured by parent-rating and performance-based instruments, in preterm and full-term preschoolers.

Method: Children age 3 to 5 years (mean 4.4) born preterm (PT; n = 82) and full term (FT; n = 79) had monolingual (PT-M, n = 51; FT-M, n = 53) or bilingual (PT-B, n = 31; FT-B, n = 26) language exposure. Groups were similar in age, gender and race, but PT children had lower socioeconomic status (SES) than FT children. Parents completed a language questionnaire and diary and a standardized parent rating of EF skills. Children completed EF tasks that tap response inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. ANCOVA and logistic regression examined effects on EF of birth group (PT/FT), language status (M/B), and birth group by language status interaction, controlling for age and SES.

Results: Compared to children born FT, children born PT had significantly higher parent-rated EF scores and poorer performance on all but one EF task, both indicating more EF problems. No main effects of language status and no birth group by language status interactions were significant.

Conclusion: PT status was clearly associated with poorer EF skills, similar to many other studies. In this sample, bilingual exposure conferred neither an advantage nor disadvantage in the FT and PT group. This information may prove useful in counseling families of both PT and FT children about the impact of bilingual exposure on their children's cognitive skills.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have no conflict of interest relevant to this article to disclose.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean Global Executive Composite (GEC) scores as a function of birth group (preterm vs. full term) and language status (monolingual vs. bilingual). **Post-hoc comparisons showed significant difference at p < .001 between preterm monolingual and both full term monolingual and full term bilingual groups after correction for multiple comparisons.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Shin HB, Ortman JM. Federal Forecasters Conference. Washington, DC: 2011. Apr 21, Language Projections: 2010 to 2020.
    1. Ryan C. Language Use in the United States: 2011. In: Bureau UC, editor. American Community Survey Reports. 2013. ACS-22 ed.
    1. Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Luk G. Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16(4):240–250. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bialystok E. Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives. 2015;9(2):117–121. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence. 2002;8(2):71–82. - PubMed

Publication types